Wernicke's area

Wernicke's area

Approximate location of Wernicke's area highlighted in grey
Identifiers
NeuroNames ancil-252
NeuroLex ID Wernicke's area
FMA 242178

Anatomical terms of neuroanatomy

Wernicke's area (/ˈvɛərnkə/ or /ˈvɛərnki/; German: [ˈvɛʁnɪkə]), also called Wernicke's speech area, is one of the two parts of the cerebral cortex linked, since the late nineteenth century, to speech (the other is Broca's area). It is involved in the comprehension or understanding of written and spoken language (in contrast to Broca's area that is involved in the production of language). It is traditionally thought to be in Brodmann area 22, which is located in the posterior section of the superior temporal gyrus (STG) in the dominant cerebral hemisphere (which is the left hemisphere in about 95% of right handed individuals and 60% of left handed individuals). Damage caused to Wernicke's area results in receptive, fluent aphasia. This means that the person with aphasia will be able to fluently connect words, but the phrases will lack meaning. This is unlike non-fluent aphasia, in which the person will use meaningful words, but in a non-fluent, telegraphic manner.[1]

Structure

Wernicke's area is classically located in the posterior section of the superior temporal gyrus (STG) in the (most commonly) left cerebral hemisphere. This area encircles the auditory cortex on the lateral sulcus (the part of the brain where the temporal lobe and parietal lobe meet).[2] This area is neuroanatomically described as the posterior part of Brodmann area 22.

However, there is an absence of consistent definitions as to the location.[3] Some identify it with the unimodal auditory association in the superior temporal gyrus anterior to the primary auditory cortex (the anterior part of BA 22).[4] This is the site most consistently implicated in auditory word recognition by functional brain imaging experiments.[5][6] Others include also adjacent parts of the heteromodal cortex in BA 39 and BA40 in the parietal lobe.[7]

While previously thought to connect Wernicke's area and Broca's area, new research demonstrates that the arcuate fasciculus instead connects to posterior receptive areas with premotor/motor areas, and not to Broca's area.[8] Consistent with the word recognition site identified in brain imaging, the uncinate fasciculus connects anterior superior temporal regions with Broca's area.[9]

Function

Right homologous area

Research using Transcranial magnetic stimulation suggests that the area corresponding to the Wernicke’s area in the non-dominant cerebral hemisphere has a role in processing and resolution of subordinate meanings of ambiguous words—such as ‘‘river’’ when given the ambiguous word "bank." In contrast, the Wernicke's area in the dominant hemisphere processes dominant word meanings (‘‘teller’’ given ‘‘bank’’).[10]

Modern views

Neuroimaging suggests the functions earlier attributed to Wernicke's area occur more broadly in the temporal lobe and indeed happen also in Broca's area.

There are some suggestions that middle and inferior temporal gyri and basal temporal cortex reflect lexical processing ... there is consensus that the STG from rostral to caudal fields and the STS constitute the neural tissue in which many of the critical computations for speech recognition are executed ... aspects of Broca’s area (Brodmann areas 44 and 45) are also regularly implicated in speech processing.

... the range of areas implicated in speech processing go well beyond the classical language areas typically mentioned for speech; the vast majority of textbooks still state that this aspect of perception and language processing occurs in Wernicke’s area (the posterior third of the STG).[11]

Support for a broad range of speech processing areas was furthered by a recent study done at University of Rochester in which American Sign Language native speakers were subject to MRIs while interpreting sentences that identified a relationship using either syntax (relationship is determined by the word order) or inflection (relationship is determined by physical motion of "moving hands through space or signing on one side of the body"). Distinct areas of the brain were activated with the frontal cortex (associated with ability to put information into sequences) being more active in the syntax condition and the temporal lobes (associated with dividing information into its constituent parts) being more active in the inflection condition. However, these areas are not mutually exclusive and show a large amount of overlap. These findings imply that while speech processing is a very complex process, the brain may be using fairly basic, preexisting computational methods.[12]

Clinical significance

Aphasia

Wernicke's area is named after Carl Wernicke, a German neurologist and psychiatrist who, in 1874, hypothesized a link between the left posterior section of the superior temporal gyrus and the reflexive mimicking of words and their syllables that associated the sensory and motor images of spoken words.[13] He did this on the basis of the location of brain injuries that caused aphasia. Receptive aphasia in which such abilities are preserved is also known as Wernicke's aphasia. In this condition there is a major impairment of language comprehension, while speech retains a natural-sounding rhythm and a relatively normal syntax. Language as a result is largely meaningless (a condition sometimes called fluent or jargon aphasia).

While neuroimaging and lesion evidence generally support the idea that malfunction of or damage to Wernicke's area is common in people with receptive aphasia, this is not always so. Some people may use the right hemisphere for language, and isolated damage of Wernicke's area cortex (sparing white matter and other areas) may not cause severe receptive aphasia.[3][14] Even when patients with Wernicke's area lesions have comprehension deficits, these are usually not restricted to language processing alone. For example, one study found that patients with posterior lesions also had trouble understanding nonverbal sounds like animal and machine noises.[15] In fact, for Wernicke's area, the impairments in nonverbal sounds were statistically stronger than for verbal sounds.

References

  1. "Aphasia: Signs & Symptoms". American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.
  2. Kennison, Shelia (2013). Introduction to language development. Los Angeles: Sage.
  3. 1 2 Bogen JE, Bogen GM (1976). "Wernicke's region—Where is it?". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 280: 834–43. Bibcode:1976NYASA.280..834B. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1976.tb25546.x. PMID 1070943.
  4. Démonet JF, Chollet F, Ramsay S, Cardebat D, Nespoulous JL, Wise R, Rascol A, Frackowiak R (December 1992). "The anatomy of phonological and semantic processing in normal subjects". Brain. 115 (Pt 6): 1753–68. doi:10.1093/brain/115.6.1753. PMID 1486459.
  5. DeWitt I, Rauschecker JP (2012). "Phoneme and word recognition in the auditory ventral stream". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 109 (8): E505–E514. Bibcode:2012PNAS..109E.505D. doi:10.1073/pnas.1113427109. PMC 3286918Freely accessible. PMID 22308358.
  6. DeWitt I, Rauschecker JP (2013). "Wernicke's area revisited: parallel streams and word processing". Brain Lang. 127 (2): 181–91. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2013.09.014. PMC 4098851Freely accessible. PMID 24404576.
  7. Mesulam MM (June 1998). "From sensation to cognition". Brain. 121 (Pt 6): 1013–52. doi:10.1093/brain/121.6.1013. PMID 9648540.
  8. Bernal B, Ardila A (September 2009). "The role of the arcuate fasciculus in conduction aphasia". Brain. 132 (Pt 9): 2309–16. doi:10.1093/brain/awp206. PMID 19690094.
  9. Saur D, Kreher BW, Schnell S, Kümmerer D, Kellmeyer P, Vry MS, Umarova R, Musso M, Glauche V, Abel S, Huber W, Rijntjes M, Hennig J, Weiller C (November 2008). "Ventral and dorsal pathways for language". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 105 (46): 18035–18040. Bibcode:2008PNAS..10518035S. doi:10.1073/pnas.0805234105. PMC 2584675Freely accessible. PMID 19004769.
  10. Harpaz Y, Levkovitz Y, Lavidor M (October 2009). "Lexical ambiguity resolution in Wernicke's area and its right homologue". Cortex. 45 (9): 1097–103. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2009.01.002. PMID 19251255.
  11. Poeppel D, Idsardi WJ, van Wassenhove V (March 2008). "Speech perception at the interface of neurobiology and linguistics". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 363 (1493): 1071–86. doi:10.1098/rstb.2007.2160. PMC 2606797Freely accessible. PMID 17890189.
  12. Newman AJ, Supalla T, Hauser P, Newport EL, Bavelier D (2010). "Dissociating neural subsystems for grammar by contrasting word order and inflection". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 107 (16): 7539–44. Bibcode:2010PNAS..107.7539N. doi:10.1073/pnas.1003174107. PMC 2867749Freely accessible. PMID 20368422.
  13. Wernicke K. (1995). "The aphasia symptom-complex: A psychological study on an anatomical basis (1875)". In Paul Eling. Reader in the History of Aphasia: From sasi(Franz Gall to). 4. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Pub Co. pp. 69–89. ISBN 90-272-1893-5.
  14. Dronkers NF.; Redfern B B.; Knight R T. (2000). "The neural architecture of language disorders". In Bizzi, Emilio; Gazzaniga, Michael S. The New cognitive neurosciences (2nd ed.). Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. pp. 949–58. ISBN 0-262-07195-9.
  15. Saygin AP, Dick F, Wilson SM, Dronkers NF, Bates E (2003). "Neural resources for processing language and environmental sounds: evidence from aphasia". Brain. 126 (Pt 4): 928–45. doi:10.1093/brain/awg082. PMID 12615649.
Wikimedia Commons has media related to Wernicke's area.
This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the 6/7/2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.