Socialization of animals

Socialization of animals is the process of training animals to be kept by humans in close relationships, especially cats and working dogs.

Ferality

Feral animals can be socialized with varying degrees of success. Feral children are children who lack socially accepted communication skills. Reports of feral children, such as those cited by Kingsley Davis, have largely been shown to be exaggerations, or complete fabrications, with regards to the specific lack of particular skills; for example, bipedalism.

Cats

For example, the cat returns readily to a feral state if it has not been socialized properly in its young life. A feral cat usually acts defensively. People often unknowingly own one and think it is merely "unfriendly."

Socializing cats older than six months can be very difficult. It is often said that they cannot be socialized. This is not true, but the process takes two to four years of diligent food bribes and handling, and mostly on the cat's terms. Eventually the cat may be persuaded to be comfortable with humans and the indoor environment.

Kittens learn to be feral either from their mothers or through bad experiences. They are more easily socialized when under six months of age. Socializing is done by keeping them confined in a small room (i.e. bathroom) and handling them for 3 or more hours each day. There are three primary methods for socialization, used individually or in combination. The first method is to simply hold and pet the cat, so it learns that such activities are not uncomfortable. The second is to use food bribes. The final method is to distract the cat with toys while handling them. The cat may then be gradually introduced to larger spaces. It is not recommended to let the cat back outside because that may cause it to revert to its feral state. The process of socialization often takes three weeks to three months for a kitten.

Animal shelters either foster feral kittens to be socialized or kill them outright. The feral adults are usually killed or euthanized, due to the large time commitment, but some shelters and vets will spay or neuter and vaccinate a feral cat and then return it to the wild.

Socialized dogs can interact with other non-aggressive dogs of any size and shape and understand how to communicate.

Dogs

In domesticated dogs, the process of socialization begins even before the puppy's eyes open. Socialization refers to both its ability to interact acceptably with humans and its understanding of how to communicate successfully with other dogs. If the mother is fearful of humans or of her environment, she can pass along this fear to her puppies. For most dogs, however, a mother who interacts well with humans is the best teacher that the puppies can have. In addition, puppies learn how to interact with other dogs by their interaction with their mother and with other adult dogs in the house.

A mother's attitude and tolerance of her puppies will change as they grow older and become more active. For this reason most experts today recommend leaving puppies with their mother until at least 8 to 10 weeks of age. This gives them a chance to experience a variety of interactions with their mother, and to observe her behavior in a range of situations.

The critical period of socialization commences when they are approximately three weeks old and will continue until they are twelve to fourteen weeks old, during which they move to the next stage of development, the juvenile period.[1] This period of socialization is the time when puppies form social bonds, learn to explore and learn when/how to base fear.[2] Additionally, this stage consists of teaching them how to appropriately react and habituate to environmental changes in preparation for adulthood.[3] Habituation is the process when a puppy has gotten used to stimuli in their environment and therefore ignores it, deeming it non-threatening.[4] The puppy’s future personality will be greatly influenced during the socialization period. Their temperament and character is developed throughout this period as well, which will last for the duration of their life. During the socialization stage, all five senses are being stimulated by exposure and desensitization of sights, sounds, tastes, smells and touch of things around them.[5]

It is critical that human interaction takes place frequently and calmly from the time the puppies are born, from simple, gentle handling to the mere presence of humans in the vicinity of the puppies, performing everyday tasks and activities. As the puppies grow older, socialization occurs more readily the more frequently they are exposed to other dogs, other people, and other situations. Dogs who are well socialized from birth, with both dogs and other species (especially people), are much less likely to be aggressive or to suffer from fear-biting.

The cognitional development of puppies can be affected when the critical period of socialization is disrupted. Physiological consequences of this period not occurring can lead to puppies maturing to adults who are not able to react appropriately to new environments, situations or people.[6] Additionally, behavioural issues can arise. Aggression, avoidance and fear are just some of the implications that can arise from a puppy not achieving the critical period of socialization.[7] It is estimated that one in four adult dogs have at least one behavioural problem.[4] Dogs are the animal which is typically the most closely attached to humans, developing tightly wound relationships with people.[8] Therefore, it is crucial for the safety of both parties that there is adequate training in place.

Dogs experience socialization through the critical period of socialization in two main types: active and passive.[5] Active implies the intended socialization of humans introducing their puppy to something/someone new (i.e. at obedience class or a ride in the car). However, passive involves socialization of the puppy to something/someone new in which they have done so unintentionally themselves (i.e. insects found in the backyard while exploring or items found while running around the house).[5]

Dogs will often learn two ways; by association and consequence.[7] Learning by association is classified as classical conditioning, while learning by consequence is called operant conditioning. With puppy socialization, classical conditioning involves pairing something they love with something within the environment.[7] Additionally, operant conditioning involves the puppy learning to do something to achieve getting what they want.[7] These two learning types can occur simultaneously with a puppy having the ability to learn both an internal and external response to a stimulus. In contrast, a puppy can also demonstrate methods of actively avoiding a situation they do not enjoy.[7]

Socialization experiments

Using puppies

The researchers, Fox and Stelzner did a socialization experiment on puppies of the same species. The major finding of this experiment is the fact that the puppies weaned from the mother at a later age (12 weeks) have better socialization skills. This proves that the beginning of a puppy's life is a very important time for socialization and will affect their social tendencies for the rest of their lives.[9]

Hennessey, Morris, and Linden conducted a socialization experiment using inmates as handlers of the shelter dogs being studied. These researchers found that the dogs in the experimental group did not jump on and bark at unfamiliar humans as much as the dogs in the control group did. The socialized dogs also showed to be more responsive to commands than did the dogs in the control group. The researchers believe that through more effective socialization, more dogs can be adopted from shelters.[10]

Battagalia claims that there are 3 important periods during the first year of life for a puppy. Her research shows that if puppies experience stimulation from humans during the first few months of their lives, they will be less likely to feel stress in their adult lives and they will be better socialized. She claims this is a very important thing for dog breeders to know to improve the success of their puppy's future lives.[11]

Using dog and wolf puppies

A study done by the six researchers, Topál, Gácsi, Miklósi, Zirányi, Kubinyi, and Csányi compares the inherent social tendencies between dog puppies and wolf puppies. What these researchers found was that dog puppies showed attachment to their owners and showed to be more responsive to their owner than an unfamiliar person. The wolf puppies did not show to be more responsive to their owners than to an unfamiliar human. Researchers concluded that this is a genetic difference between species.[12]

These six researchers attempted to answer the question, "Why?" in their next experiment. They found that dogs were more successful in finding hidden food and in completing a learned task than wolves were. The most prominent observation these researchers made was that the dogs would look into the eyes of the human as if looking for a clue and the wolves would not look at the faces of the humans. They concluded that this is a genetic difference between the two species as a result of evolution.[13]

Monkeys

Monkeys are often studied because of the close evolutionary relationship between monkeys and humans.

Socialization experiments

Bernstein and Ehardt conducted an experiment on aggressive behavior of rhesus monkeys. They found that the monkeys showed more aggression towards kin than non-kin, mostly from older kin to younger kin. This supports the hypothesis that aggression is used in socialization and correction of inappropriate behaviors in the immature monkeys. If the aggression is no more severe than is needed to correct the behavior, it can improve the survival rate of all the relatives.[14]

In 1980, Berman researched the mother-infant relationship of rhesus monkeys both in the wild and in captivity. She found many similarities between the two parenting styles but some minor differences. The captive mother has been described as more protective and less willing to let their infant out of their control. The difference that likely has to most effect on this relationship is, the wild monkey has kin around which helps with the socialization of the infant, and the mother in captivity does not. This shows how environmental factors can affect the early socialization of infant rhesus monkeys.[15]

Socialization at a young age has been seen to affect sexual behavior in the adult rhesus monkey, in a study done by, Gold, Wallen, and Goldfoot. This is not seen as prominently in rats, and other small animals as it is in primates. The monkeys have difficulty acting normally even when a sexual opportunity presents itself, this is due to the fact that they have affectional disorders that they do not often overcome.[16]

It is very difficult to study the lives of isolated children so researchers have turned to studying the effect of total isolation on rhesus monkeys. Completely isolated monkey's first response to stimulus is fear or aggression. They do not learn any normal socio-emotional skills. When these monkeys are able to come in contact with a group, they do not know how to interact and would not be able to survive in a group.[17]

See also

References

  1. Battaglia, Carmen L (2009). "Periods of Early Development and the Effects of Stimulation and Social Experiences in the Canine". . Journal of Veterinary Behavior: Clinical Applications and Research. 4 (5): 203–210.
  2. Lord, Kathryn (2012). "A comparison of the sensory development of wolves (Canis lupus lupus) and dogs (Canis lupus familiaris)". International Journal of Behavioural Biology: Ethology. doi:10.1111/eth.12044.
  3. Merck Veterinary Manual. Merck Manuals http://www.merckvetmanual.com/mvm/index.html. Missing or empty |title= (help)
  4. 1 2 Appleby, David. "Puppy Socialisation and Habituation (Part 1) Why is it Necessary?". Association of Pet Behaviour Counsellors (APBC).
  5. 1 2 3 "What is Puppy Socialization Anyway?". Dog Training Central.
  6. Donovan, Jane. "Puppy Socialization: When, Why and How to do it Right". American Kennel Club.
  7. 1 2 3 4 5 Bindoff, Aidan. "How to Socialize Your Puppy". Karen Pryor Clicker Training.
  8. Scott, J.P. (1958). "Critical Periods in the Development of Social Behavior in Puppies". Psychosomatic Medicine: Journal of Biobehavioral Medicine. 20 (1).
  9. Fox, Stelzner, M.W., D. "The Effects of Early Experience on the development of inter and intraspecies social relationships in the dog" (PDF). sciencedirect.org. Retrieved 18 February 2015.
  10. Hennessey, Morris, Linden, Michael, Angel, Fran. "Evaluation of the Effects of a Socialization program in a Prison on Behavior and pituitary- adrenal hormone levels of shelter dogs" (PDF). sciencedirect.org. Retrieved 18 February 2015.
  11. Battagalia, Carmen. "Periods of early development and the effects of stimulation and social experience in the canine" (PDF). sciencedirect.org. Retrieved 19 February 2015.
  12. Topál, Gácsi, Miklósi, Zirányi, Kubinyi, and Csányi. "Attachment to humans: A comparative study on hand-reared wolves and differently socialized dog puppies" (PDF). sciencedirect.org. Retrieved 18 February 2015.
  13. Topál, Gácsi, Miklósi, Zirányi, Kubinyi, and Csányi. "A simple reason for a big difference: Wolves do not look back at humans but dogs do" (PDF). sciencedirect.org. Retrieved 18 February 2015.
  14. Bernstein, Ehardt, Irwin, Carolyn. "The influence of kinship and socialization on aggressive behaviour in rhesus monkeys" (PDF). sciencedirect.org. Retrieved 19 February 2015.
  15. Berman, Carol. "MOTHER-INFANT RELATIONSHIPS AMONG FREE-RANGING RHESUS MONKEYS ON CAYO SANTIAGO: A COMPARISON WITH CAPTIVE PAIRS" (PDF). sciencedirect.org. Retrieved 19 February 2015.
  16. Goy, Wallen, Goldfoot, Robert, Kim, David. "Social Factors Affecting the Development of Mounting Behavior in Male Rhesus Monkeys". Advances in Behavioral Biology: 223–247. doi:10.1007/978-1-4684-3069-1_10. Retrieved 19 February 2015.
  17. Harlow, Dodsworth, Harlow, Harry, Robert, Margaret (1965). "Total social isolation in monkeys". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 54: 90–7. PMC 285801Freely accessible. PMID 4955132.
This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the 12/2/2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.