Psychological adaptation

A psychological adaptation is a functional, cognitive or behavioral trait that benefits an organism in its environment. Psychological adaptations fall under the scope of Evolved Psychological Mechanisms (EPM), however, the two are not synonymous. Psychological adaptations refer to the functional cognitive traits that increase the fitness of an organism, while EPMs refer to any psychological mechanism that developed through the processes of evolution. Additional EPMs include both the bi-product traits of a species’ evolutionary development (see spandrels), as well as the vestigial traits that no longer benefit the species’ fitness. Some evolutionists consider vestigial traits as adaptations, even though they no longer have adaptive functionality. The principles of psychological adaptation rely on Darwin’s theory of evolution and are important to the fields of evolutionary psychology, biology, and cognitive science.

Darwinian theory

Charles Darwin’s theory of evolution dictates that adaptations are traits that arise from the selective pressures a species faces. Adaptations must benefit either an organism’s survival or reproduction, and are passed down to the next generation through this process of natural selection.[1] Psychological adaptions are those adaptive traits that we consider cognitive or behavioral. These can include conscious social strategies, feeling responses(guilt, anger, etc.), or the most innate instincts. Evolutionary psychologists consider a number of factors in what determines a psychological adaptation, such as functionality, complexity, efficiency, and universality.[1]

Evolved adaptation vs learned behaviour

An area of disagreement arises between evolutionary psychologists, cognitive scientists and behaviourists on where to draw the line on what is considered a psychological adaptation, and what is considered a learned behaviour.[2] Where behaviourism explains a conscious behaviour as a conditioned response, cognitivism suggest that behaviours arise from a psychological adaptation that institutes a preference for that response. Evolutionary psychology proposes that the human psychology consists primarily of psychological adaptations, which is opposed by the tabula rasa or blank slate model of human psychology. Early behaviourists, like B.F. Skinner, tended to the blank slate model and argued that innate behaviors and instincts were few, some suggesting that the only innate behavior was the ability to learn.[2] On the other hand, Steven Pinker presents the cognitivist perspective in his book, The Blank Slate, in which he challenges the tabula rasa models and argues that human behaviour is shaped by psychological adaptations.[3] Modern human sexual preferences show this difference in opinion, with behaviourists arguing that attraction has conditioning influences, such as from the media or cultural norms, while others argue it is based on psychological adaptations.[4] However, sexual preferences are a difficult subject to test due to the amount of variance and flexibility exhibited in human mate choice.[5] A hybrid resolution to psychological adaptations and learned behaviours refers to an adaptation as the species’ capacity for a certain behavior, while each individual organism still needs to be conditioned to exhibit that behaviour. This approach is inherent in linguist and cognitive scientist Noam Chomsky's model of human language.[6] The capacity for language in humans is a psychological adaptation that includes language necessary brain structures and disposition for language acquisition (traits which are passed down through our genetic code), however, a newborn baby lacks any instantiation of language at birth, and must instead learn over time.

Sexual selection

There are many examples of psychological adaptations concerning human mating behavior. There is extensive evidence that incest avoidance, which is a tendency to avoid sexual intercourse with close relatives, is a psychological adaptation.[1] Incest adaptation can be seen cross-culturally in humans, and is evident in wild animals. Evolutionary psychologists argue that since there is a large chance of producing children with severe disabilities when mating with relatives, incest avoidance offers an increase in fitness regarding offspring survival.[1] Sexual jealousy is another behavior observed in human and non-human animals that appears to be instinctual. Heuristic problem solving and consistent preference for behavioral patterns are considered by some evolutionary psychologists to be psychological adaptations.[7] For example, a tendency for females to change their sexual strategies when faced with developmental pressures such as an absent father may be the result of a psychological adaptation.

Psychological adaptation in males

Human males have developed psychological adaptations, which make them attractive to the opposite sex in order to increase their reproductive success. Evolutionarily, it pays for a male to be polygynous - to have a number of female partners at once - because it means they can create more offspring at once, as they don’t have to invest any time in carrying a baby. Examples of some of these other adaptations include strategies to entice females, strategies to retain a partner and the desire for short-term relationships.

It has been researched that humour is sexually selected and acts as a fitness indicator.[8] The production of humour increases mate value in men, and so women seek males with a good sense of humour. In turn, males have developed an adaptation in which they produce humour to attract women.

Human males have developed an adaptation in which they find women more attractive if they show cues of fertility, such as a good hip-to-waist ratio. Women with a hip-to-ratio of 0.7 are considered more attractive to males than those with a ratio of 0.8, who are considered to have a more masculine figure.[9] This is because they are perceived to be able to have children more and to be more fertile and healthy.

Males have developed behaviours that help them to retain a mate, in order to enhance reproductive success in long-term relationships. Examples are intersexual manipulations which involves the male manipulating the way his partner views their current relationship and to repulse her from other relationships.[10] He could do this by enhancing his own value or decreasing the value of other males. In extreme cases, some men have developed intersexual adaptations that restrict their partner from interacting with other males, including the use of violence. By doing this, women are less likely to stray from her current relationship, even if it is due to fear. On the other hand, intrasexual manipulations are used to reduce any other options for the females, which could include decreasing their partner’s value or make it clear to other males that the female is ‘their’s’ by using possessive techniques such as holding her hand in public.[11]

With regards to parental investment, males are much more wary when investing in offspring as they cannot guarantee that the child is their’s.[12] Therefore, as an adaption, males tend to only invest in offspring if there are high levels of commitment and if they were produced in a long-term relationship as opposed to short-term relationships.

Short-term mating

Human males have also developed an adaptation in which they have a desire for short-term relationships more than females do.[13] This is because men hardly have any investment obligation, whereas a female has to carry a child for nine months if she was to fall pregnant after the sexual encounter. Evolutionarily, it is thought that males have a desire to reproduce as much as they can, and short-term relationships are a good way to inseminate many women with his sperm in order for his genes to continue through generations. There is much evidence for how this short-term mating has evolved psychologically for males, beginning with the desire for a variety of sex partners. It seems that a larger percentage of men, in every culture of the world, desire more than one sex partner in one month compared to women.[14] Furthermore, men are more likely than women to have sexual intercourse with someone having known them for only one hour, one day, one week or one month.[15]

However, there are some adaptive problems in short-term mating that men must solve. One of these problems is avoiding commitment and women who might not have sex with the male until they have a signal of commitment or investment. This would reduce the number of partners a male could pursue and succeed with.[16]

Psychological adaptation in females

Female sex-specific adaptations provide evidence of special design for the purpose of increasing fitness and in turn, reproductive success.[17] For example, mate choice, rape aversion tactics and pregnancy sickness are all female-specific psychological adaptations, identified through empirical research, found to increase genetic contributions through survival and reproduction.[18]

Mate-choice as an adaptation

A psychological adaptation for the purpose of reproductive success can be seen in female mate choice. David Buss, an evolutionary psychologist, examines the fundamental principles of selection pressures that create human mate preferences in his contribution to the publication The Adapted Mind.[19] Females have evolved psychological procedures that affect mating decisions in relation to certain male physical attributes and behaviours.[20] Robert Trivers, an evolutionary biologist, outlines the evolutionary basis of these preferences in relation to parental investment and sexual selection.[21] He proposes that females have adapted a preference to mate with males who display both an ability and willingness to invest vital resources for the survival of the female and her offspring. Research suggests females are able to use external cues displayed by males such as territory or physical possessions. For example, women are able to evaluate the long-term presence of testosterone in men by observing facial testosterone cues.[22] Testosterone stimulates craniofacial development and results in a squarer jaw and consequently, a more masculine appearance. Women in the fertile phase of their menstrual cycle perceive masculine faces as healthier and more attractive than feminine male faces.[23] Females show a psychological adaptation to detect mate quality using these hormonal cues which display the male’s fitness and reproductive value. Males who display testosterone cues show a female that they are able to offset the high physiological costs such as immunosuppressant effects.[24]

Rape avoidance

Research proposes that women have evolved psychological mechanisms specifically designed to motivate rape-avoidance behaviours or strategies.[25] This is because rape poses severe costs for the female such as pregnancy, physical harm, injury or death, relationship abandonment and self-esteem depletion.[26] The greatest cost to the female is the circumvention of her mate choice, which threatens reproductive success, resulting in the possession of adaptations in response.[27] Evidence suggests that a number of female-specific traits have evolved in order to reduce the risks associated with experiencing rape. The body-guard hypothesis[28] proposes that rape-avoidance drives women’s mate preferences for physically or dominant males. Women may also form groups with men and women as a protective alliance against potential rapists.[29] Psychological pain experienced following rape is also identified as an adaptive process designed to focus the female on the social circumstances surrounding the rape for future prevention.[30]

Evidence for this as an adaptation can be seen in reproductive-aged women who are found to experience more psychological pain following rape due to an increased risk of conception.[31] Research also suggests that women in the fertile phase of their menstrual cycle perform fewer risky behaviours that could potentially result in the risk of rape.[32] Women’s capacity to resist rape also changes relative to their menstrual cycle; females in the fertile phase show an increase in handgrip strength when placed in a threatening, sexually coercive scenario.[33] Susceptibility to signs of a male’s coerciveness is also identified to be better in fertile women.[34]

Pregnancy sickness

One psychological adaptation found solely in women is pregnancy sickness.[35] This is an adaptation resulting from natural selection for the purpose of avoiding toxic-containing foods during pregnancy. Margaret Profet, an evolutionary biologist, provides evidence for this adaptation in a literature review on pregnancy sickness.[36] Particular plant foods, whilst unharmful to adults, can contain toxins (e.g. teratogens) that are dangerous for developing embryos and can potentially cause birth defects such as facial asymmetry. Evidence lies in the finding that women who experience more extreme cases of pregnancy sickness tend to be less likely to miscarry or have babies with birth defects. This fits the criteria for an adaptation as it enhances fitness and increases reproductive success – it results in greater fertility of the mother and contributes to the health of the developing embryo.

Researchers dispute whether this is actually a psychological adaptation, however evidence advocates it is the result of strong selective pressures in our hereditary past. For example, the toxins are found only in natural wild plant foods, not processed foods in our modern-day environment. Furthermore, pregnant women experiencing sickness have been found to avoid particular bitter or pungent smelling foods, potentially containing toxins. Pregnancy induced sickness only typically occurs 3 weeks after conception, around the time when the embryo has started forming major organs and is therefore at the highest risk.[37] It is also a cross-cultural universal adaptation, a suggestion it is an innate mechanism.

See also

References

  1. 1 2 3 4 Schmitt, D.P. & Pilcher, J.J. (2004). Evaluating evidence of psychological adaptation: How do we know one when we see one? Psychological Science, 15, 643-649. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00734.x http://pss.sagepub.com/content/15/10/643.full?hwshib2=authn%3A1480047764%3A20161123%253A4525d97a-185a-4a2d-8ca1-05f9b9d929e4%3A0%3A0%3A0%3AcXdSmikoeO3akK3bM8nOqw%3D%3D
  2. 1 2 Buss, D.M. (2004) Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind. Evolutionary Psychology 3rd Ed. Boston.
  3. Pinker, Steven. (2002) The blank slate: The modern denial of human nature. Allen Lane, London. Print
  4. Fisher, R.A. (1915) The evolution of sexual preference. The Eugenics Review, 7(3):184-192. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2987134/
  5. Miller, G.F. (1997) Mate choice: From sexual cues to cognitive adaptations. Ciba Foundation Symposium, 208:71-87.
  6. Lenneberg, E. H., Chomsky, N., & Marx, O. (1967). Biological foundations of language (Vol. 68). New York: Wiley. http://tocs.ulb.tu-darmstadt.de/123135893.pdf
  7. Buss, D.M., Kruger, D., Kurzban, R. et al. (2009) Evolutionary theory and psychology. Psychological Science Agenda, Science Briefs. http://www.apa.org/science/about/psa/2009/05/sci-brief.aspx#Machery
  8. Greengross, G. (2014). Male production of humour produced by sexually selected psychological adaptations. In Shackelford, V. A & Shackelford, T. K. (2014). Evolutionary perspectives on human sexual psychology and behaviour(173-196). New York:Springer.
  9. Singh, D. (1993). Adaptive significance of female physical attraction: Role of waist-to-hip ratio. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 293-307.
  10. Starratt, V. G., & Alesia, M. N. (2014). Male adaptations to retain a mate. In Shackelford, V. A., & Shackelford, T. K. (2014). Evolutionary perspectives on human sexual psychology and behaviour 197-205. New York:Springer.
  11. Buss, D. M. (1988). From vigilance to violence: Tactics of mate retention in American undergraduates. Ethology and Sociobiology, 9, 291-317.
  12. Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In Campbell, B, (Ed.), Sexual selection and the descent of man, 1871-1971, 136-179. London: Heinemann.
  13. Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An evolutionary perspective on human mating. Psychological Review, 100, 204-232.
  14. Schmitt, D. P., and 118 members of the International Sexuality Description Project. (2003). Universal sex differences in the desire for sexual variety: Tests from 52 nations, 6 continents, and 13 islands. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 85-104.
  15. Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An evolutionary perspective on human mating. Psychological Review, 100, 204-232
  16. Jonason, P. K., & Buss, D. M. (2012). Avoiding entangling commitments: Tactics for implementing a short-term mating strategy. Personality and Individual Differences, 52,606-610.
  17. Williams, G.C. (1996). Adaptation and natural selection. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
  18. Alcock, J. (1993). Animal behaviour: An evolutionary approach. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.
  19. Buss, D. M. (1995). Mate Preference Mechanisms: Consequences for Partner Choice and Intrasexual Competition. In J.H. Barkow, L. Cosmides & J. Tooby. (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. (pp. 249-266). New York: Oxford University Press.
  20. Symons, D. (1979). The evolution of the human mind. New York: Oxford University Press.
  21. Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B. Campbell (Eds.), Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man. (pp. 136–179). Aldinc: Chicago.
  22. Waynforth, D. (2011). Mate choice and sexual selection. In V. Swami (Eds.), Evolutionary Psychology: a critical introduction. (pp. 107-130). West Sussex: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.
  23. Rhodes, G., Chan, J., Zebrowitz, L. A., & Simmons, L. W. (2003). Does sexual dimorphism in human faces signal health?. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 270, S93-S95.
  24. Al-Afaleq, A. & Homeida, A. (1998). Effects of low doses of oestradiol, testosterone, and dihydrotestosterone on the immune response of broiler chicks. Immunopharacology and Immunotoxicology, 20, 315-327.
  25. McKibbin, W. F., Shackelford, T. K., Goetz, A. T., Bates, V. M., Starratt, V. G., & Miner, E. J. (2009). Development and initial psychometric assessment of the Rape Avoidance Inventory. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 336–340.
  26. Thornhill, R., & Palmer, C. T. (2000). A natural history of rape. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  27. Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B. Campbell (Ed.), Sexual selection and the descent of man: 1871-1971. (pp. 136–179). Chicago: Aldine.
  28. Wilson, M., & Mesnick, S. L. (1997). An empirical test of the bodyguard hypothesis. In Feminism and evolutionary biology (pp. 505-511). Springer US.
  29. Smuts, B. (1992). Male aggression against women. Human Nature, 3, 1-44.
  30. Thornhill, N. W., & Thornhill, R. (1991). An evolutionary analysis of psychological pain following human (Homo sapiens) rape: IV. The effect of the nature of the sexual assault. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 105, 243.
  31. Thornhill, N. W., & Thornhill, R. (1990). An evolutionary analysis of psychological pain following rape:: I. The effects of victim's age and marital status. Ethology and Sociobiology, 11, 155-176.
  32. Chavanne, T. J., & Gallup, G. G. (1998). Variation in risk taking behavior among female college students as a function of the menstrual cycle. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19, 27-32.
  33. Petralia, S. M., & Gallup, G. G. (2002). Effects of a sexual assault scenario on handgrip strength across the menstrual cycle. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 3-10.
  34. Garver-Apgar, C. E., Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (2007). Women’s perceptions of men’s sexual coerciveness change across the menstrual cycle. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 39, 536-540.
  35. Chan, R. L., Olshan, A. F., Savitz, D. A., Herring, A. H., Daniels, J. L., Peterson, H. B. & Martin, S. L. (2010). Severity and duration of nausea and vomiting symptoms in pregnancy and spontaneous abortion. Human Reproduction, 25, 2907–12.
  36. Profet, M. (1992). Pregnancy sickness as adaptation: A deterrent to maternal ingestion of teratogens. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 327-366). New York: Oxford University Press.
  37. Profet, M. (1988). The evolution of pregnancy sickness as protection to the embryo against Pleistocene teratogens. Evolutionary Theory, 8, 177-190.

External links

This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the 12/1/2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.