Obviative

Obviative (abbreviated OBV) third person is a grammatical-person marking that distinguishes a non-salient (obviative) third-person referent from a more salient (proximate) third-person referent in a given discourse context. The obviative is sometimes referred to as the "fourth person".[1]

Comparison with other grammatical-person marking systems

In English and other European languages, the principal means of distinguishing between multiple third-person referents is using gender or (lack of) reflexive. Thus, in "she saw him," it is clear that there are two third persons because they are of different genders. In "she saw her," it is clear that there are two third persons because otherwise, one would say "she saw herself." However, "she saw her mother" is ambiguous: it could mean that she saw her own mother or that she saw someone else's mother.[2]

An obviative/proximate system has a different way of distinguishing between multiple third-person referents. When there is more than one third person named in a sentence or discourse context, the most important, salient, or topical is marked as "proximate" and any other, less salient entities are marked as "obviative." Subsequent sentences that refer to previously-named entities with pronouns or verbal inflections can then use the proximate, and obviative references that have already been established to distinguish between the two.[2]

For example, in the sentence "the quick brown fox jumped over the lazy dog," there are two third-person referents, the fox and the dog. Thus, one of them has to be proximate and the other one has to be obviative, depending on which one the speaker considers more central to the story. If the fox is the more important one, the sentence might look something like "the quick brown fox-PROX jumped-PROX>OBV the lazy dog-OBV," where PROX>OBV is verbal inflection indicating a proximate subject acting on an obviative object. In that case, a subsequent sentence "and then PROX went-PROX away" would mean that the fox went away.

On the other hand, if the dog is the more important one, the sentence might look something like "the quick brown fox-OBV jumped-OBV>PROX the lazy dog-PROX," where OBV>PROX is verbal inflection indicating an obviative subject acting on a proximate object. In that case, the same subsequent sentence "and then PROX went-PROX away" would mean instead that the dog went away. By contrast, an equivalent subsequent sentence in English, such as "and then he went away," would not necessarily indicate whether "he" is the fox or the dog.

An analogy that has been used to explain obviation is that the proximate is the entity in the "spotlight," and any other, obviative entities are out of the spotlight or "hangers-on."[3]

Geography

North America

Obviate/proximate distinctions are common in some indigenous language families in northern North America. Algonquian languages are perhaps best known for obviation, but the feature occurs also in some Salishan languages and in the language isolate Kutenai as well as in the more southern Keresan languages.[4]

Africa

Obviative markers are used in Africa in some Nilo-Saharan and Niger–Congo languages.[5]

Asia

Obviation has also been attested in the Northeast Caucasian Ingush language in Asia.[6]

Cross-linguistic patterns

Examples

Ojibwe

The following is a typical example of obviate/proximate morphology in the Eastern dialect of the Algonquian Obijwe in which the obviative is marked on nouns and demonstratives and reflected in pronominal verb affixes:

Maabadashshkinwewgii-bwaadaawii-bi-yaanidmyagi-nishnaabenwaa-bi-nsigwaajin
maabadashoshkinaweo-gii-bawaad-am-nwii-bi-ayaa-ini-dmayagi-nishanaabe-anx-wii-bi-nis-igo-waa-d-in
thisEMPyoung.man3-PAST-dream-3INAN-OBVFUT-coming-be.at-OBV-3foreign-people-OBVREL-FUT-coming-kill-INV-3-OBV

'Then this (PROX) young man (PROX) dreamed (PROX) that foreigners (OBV) would come (OBV) to kill (OBV) them (PROX).'

That example shows that the proximate referent need not necessarily be the subject of a clause.[4]

Potawatomi

Potawatomi (an Algonquian Language) is notable for having two degrees of obviation, "obviation" and "further obviation." "Further obviation" is rare, but when it occurs, a "further obviative" referent, deemed to be even less salient than the obviative referent, can be marked by an additional obviative suffix. The following is the sole example to appear in the literature on Potawatomi:

ktokmamnannun
our (12) chief(s) (3″)[7]

Charles Hockett[8] posited he following example, but he never checked it to see if it was grammatical:

waposowaposo-nwaposo-n-un
rabbitrabbit-OBVrabbit-OBV-OBV
/proximate//obviative//further obviative/[9]

Ingush

Obviation in the Ingush, a heavily dependent-marking language, is an exception to the generalization that the obviative occurs in head-marking languages. Obviation is not overtly marked in Ingush but is implied, as certain constructions are impossible unless one referent has salience over another.

For example, if a non-subject-referent has salience over the subject and precedes the other co-referent, reflexivisation (normally used only when there is a coreferent to the subject) is possible. That is shown in the example below whose non-subject-referent appears to have salience over the subject:

Muusaajnashiizhwaliit'y-weaxar
Musa-DAT3S-RFL-GENdogon-bark-WITNESSED PAST

'Musa's dog barked at him.'

If the subject is salient ("proximate"), on the other hand, the subject's possessor does not antecede the third-person object, and the possession must be indirectly implicated as follows:

Muusaasiesaguoliex
Musawife-ERGseek

'Musa's wife is looking for him.' (Literally, 'The wife is looking for Musa.')[6]

See also

References

  1. Kibort, Anna. "Person." Grammatical Features. 7 January 2008. Retrieved on 2009-10-25.
  2. 1 2 "Spec Tech: Wait, who hit who?". February 17, 2011. Retrieved April 7, 2015.
  3. Quinn, Conor McDonough (2006). "Algonquian grammar without all the grammar." (PDF). Retrieved April 7, 2015.
  4. 1 2 3 Mithun, Marianne. The languages of Native North America. 76-68.
  5. 1 2 Gregersen, Edgar A. Language in Africa: an introductory survey. New York: Gordon and Breach Science Publishers Inc, 1977. 51-52.
  6. 1 2 3 "The Scientific Interest of Ingush - Section 5, Obviation" University of California, Berkeley (Unpublished). Retrieved on 2009-10-29.
  7. Hockett, Charles. Potawatomi II: Derivation, Person Prefixes, and Nouns. International Journal of American Linguistics. 1948. 14:63-74.
  8. Hockett, Charles. What Algonquian is Really Like. International Journal of American Linguistics. 1966. 32:59-73.
  9. Schlenker, Philippe. Propositional Attitudes and Indexicality: A Cross-Categorial Approach. University of Southern California, 1994. 44-45.
7. Aissen, Judith. 1997. On the syntax of obviation. Language 73:4.705-50.
This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the 9/3/2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.