Human multitasking

Human multitasking is an apparent human ability to perform more than one task, or activity, over a short period of time. An example of multitasking is taking phone calls while typing an email and reading a book. Multitasking can result in time wasted due to human context switching and apparently causing more errors due to insufficient attention. Studies have shown that it is impossible to focus on more than one task at a time. However, if one is proficient at one of the tasks at hand, then it is possible to do these tasks.

Etymology

Main article: Computer multitasking

The first published use of the word "multitask" appeared in an IBM paper describing the capabilities of the IBM System/360 in 1965.[1] In this context, "multitasking" refers to the ability of a computer to apparently process several tasks, or computer jobs, concurrently.[2] The term has since been applied to human tasks.

Research

Since the 1960s, psychologists have conducted experiments on the nature and limits of human multitasking. The simplest experimental design used to investigate human multitasking is the so-called psychological refractory period effect. Here, people are asked to make separate responses to each of two stimuli presented close together in time. An extremely general finding is a slowing in responses to the second-appearing stimulus.[3]

Researchers have long suggested that there appears to be a processing bottleneck preventing the brain from working on certain key aspects of both tasks at the same time (e.g., (Gladstones, Regan & Lee 1989) (Pashler 1994)). Many researchers believe that the cognitive function subject to the most severe form of bottlenecking is the planning of actions and retrieval of information from memory.[4] Psychiatrist Edward M. Hallowell[5] has gone so far as to describe multitasking as a “mythical activity in which people believe they can perform two or more tasks simultaneously as effectively as one.” On the other hand, there is good evidence that people can monitor many perceptual streams at the same time, and carry out perceptual and motor functions at the same time.

Others have researched multitasking in specific domains, such as learning. Mayer and Moreno[6] studied the phenomenon of cognitive load in multimedia learning and concluded that it is difficult, if not impossible, to learn new information while engaging in multitasking. Junco and Cotten examined how multitasking affects academic success and found that students who engaged in high levels of multitasking reported significant issues with their academic work.[7] A more recent study on the effects of multitasking on academic performance found that using Facebook and text messaging while studying were negatively related to student grades, while online searching and emailing were not.[8]

The brain's role

Because the brain cannot fully focus when multitasking, people take longer to complete tasks and are predisposed to error. When people attempt to complete many tasks at one time, “or [alternate] rapidly between them, errors go way up and it takes far longer—often double the time or more—to get the jobs done than if they were done sequentially,” states Meyer.[9] This is largely because “the brain is compelled to restart and refocus”.[10] A study by Meyer and David Kieras found that in the interim between each exchange, the brain makes no progress whatsoever. Therefore, multitasking people not only perform each task less suitably, but lose time in the process.

According to a study done by Jordan Grafman, chief of the cognitive neuroscience section at the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, “the most anterior part [of the brain] allows [a person] to leave something when it’s incomplete and return to the same place and continue from there,” while Brodmann Area 10, a part of the brain’s frontal lobes, is important for establishing and attaining long-term goals.[9] Focusing on multiple dissimilar tasks at once forces the brain to process all activity in its anterior. Though the brain is complex and can perform a myriad of tasks, it cannot multitask well.

Another study by René Marois, a psychologist of Vanderbilt University, discovered that the brain exhibits a “response selection bottleneck” when asked to perform several tasks at once. The brain must then decide which activity is most important, thereby taking more time. Psychologist David Meyer of the University of Michigan claims that, instead of a “bottleneck,” the brain experiences “adaptive executive control” which places priorities on each activity. These viewpoints differ in that, while bottlenecking attempts to force many thoughts through the brain at once, adaptive executive control prioritizes tasks to maintain a semblance of order. The brain better understands this order and, as psychologists such as Dr. Meyer believe, can therefore be trained to multitask.[11] It is not known exactly how the brain processes input and reacts to overstimulation.

Some research suggests that the human brain can be trained to multitask. A study published in Child Development by Monica Luciana, associate professor of psychology at the University of Minnesota, discovered that the brain’s capability of categorizing competing information continues to develop until ages sixteen and seventeen. A study by Vanderbilt University found that multitasking is largely limited by “the speed with which our prefrontal cortex processes information.” Paul E. Dux, co-author of the study, believes that this process can become faster through proper training. The study trained seven people to perform two simple tasks, either separately or together, and conducted brain scans of the participants. The individuals multitasked poorly at first but, with training, were able to adeptly perform the tasks simultaneously. Brain scans of the participants indicate that the prefrontal cortex quickened its ability to process the information, enabling the individuals to multitask more efficiently. However, the study also suggests that the brain is incapable of performing multiple tasks at one time, even after extensive training.[12] This study further indicates that, while the brain can become adept at processing and responding to certain information, it cannot truly multitask.

People have a limited ability to retain information, which worsens when the amount of information increases. For this reason people alter information to make it more memorable, such as separating a ten-digit phone number into three smaller groups or dividing the alphabet into sets of three to five letters. George Miller, former psychologist at Harvard University, believes the limits to the human brain’s capacity centers around “the number seven, plus or minus two.” An illustrative example of this is a test in which a person must repeat numbers read aloud. While two or three numbers are easily repeated, fifteen numbers becomes more difficult. The person would, on average, repeat seven correctly.[13] Brains are only capable of storing a limited amount of information in their short-term memories.

Laboratory based studies of multi-tasking indicate that one motivation for switching between tasks is to increase the time spent on the task that produces the most reward (Payne, Duggan & Neth, 2007). This reward could be progress towards an overall task goal or it could simply be the opportunity to pursue a more interesting or fun activity. Payne, Duggan and Neth (2007) found that decisions to switch task reflected either the reward provided by the current task or the availability of a suitable opportunity to switch (i.e. the completion of a subgoal). A French fMRI study published in 2010 indicated preliminary support for the hypothesis that the brain can pursue at most two goals simultaneously, one for each frontal lobe (which has a goal-oriented area).[14]

Sex differences

Although the idea that women are better multitaskers than men has been popular in the media as well in conventional thought, there is very little data available to support claims of a real sex difference. Most studies that do show any sex differences tend to find that the differences are small and inconsistent.[15]

A study by psychologist Keith Laws was widely reported in the press to have provided the first evidence of female multitasking superiority.[16][17]

In another study, females were found to perform slightly better at coordinating a primary test with a secondary test, supporting the notion that females are better at multitasking. However, the authors concluded their tests may not reflect real-life multitasking and that further research was required.[18]

Conversely, a Swedish study found that men actually outperformed women at handling multiple tasks simultaneously, with the performance gap being correlated to the female menstrual cycle.[19]

More recently, a new brain connectivity study from Penn Medicine, funded by in part by the National Institutes of Mental Health, published in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences found major differences in men and women’s neural wiring that is leading researchers to believe in popular belief that sex plays a role in multitasking skills. "[On] average, men are more likely better at learning and performing a single task at hand, like cycling or navigating directions, whereas women have superior memory and social cognition skills, making them more equipped for multitasking and creating solutions that work for a group."[20] The full text of the study can be found on the PNAS website.[21] However, this study has been widely criticized because the differences that are seen, could easily have been caused by increased head movement. Moreover, the link between the DTI data and behavioral performance is speculative. Importantly, this study contains no such evidence of any superiority in multitasking in women.

While not many recent studies have evidence of female multitasking superiority there are evolutionary explanations showing the popular belief to be true. In 1992, Silverman and Eals created a theory derived from the Hunter-Gatherer Hypothesis. Their theory says that natural selection favored hunting-related skills and resulted in a difference in task performance for genders. Men focused on one task, hunting, while women were gatherers and took care of the children at home. The idea is that over time there was a strong selection against women who could not multitask. We no longer have such rigid labor division but the natural selection that took place in primitive societies is thought to have made modern females superior multitaskers.[22]

Continuous partial attention

Author Steven Berlin Johnson describes one kind of multitasking: “It usually involves skimming the surface of the incoming data, picking out the relevant details, and moving on to the next stream. You’re paying attention, but only partially. That lets you cast a wider net, but it also runs the risk of keeping you from really studying the fish."[23] Multimedia pioneer Linda Stone coined the phrase "continuous partial attention" for this kind of processing.[24] Continuous partial attention is multitasking where things do not get studied in depth.

Rapidly increasing technology fosters multitasking because it promotes multiple sources of input at a given time. Instead of exchanging old equipment like TV, print, and music, for new equipment such as computers, the Internet, and video games, children and teens combine forms of media and continually increase sources of input.[25] According to studies by the Kaiser Family Foundation, in 1999 only 16 percent of time spent using media such as internet, television, video games, telephones, text-messaging, or e-mail was combined. In 2005, 26 percent of the time these media were used together.[11] This increase in simultaneous media usage decreases the amount of attention paid to each device. In 2005 it was found that 82 percent of American youth use the Internet by the seventh grade in school.[26] A 2005 survey by the Kaiser Family Foundation found that, while their usage of media continued at a constant 6.5 hours per day, Americans ages 8 to 18 were crowding roughly 8.5 hours’ worth of media into their days due to multitasking. The survey showed that one quarter to one third of the participants have more than one input “most of the time” while watching television, listening to music, or reading.[9] The 2007 Harvard Business Review featured Linda Stone’s idea of “continuous partial attention,” or, “constantly scanning for opportunities and staying on top of contacts, events, and activities in an effort to miss nothing”.[11] As technology provides more distractions, attention is spread among tasks more thinly.

A prevalent example of this inattention to detail due to multitasking is apparent when people talk on cellphones while driving. One study found that having an accident is four times more likely when using a cell phone while driving.[27] Another study compared reaction times for experienced drivers during a number of tasks, and found that the subjects reacted more slowly to brake lights and stop signs during phone conversations than during other simultaneous tasks.[27] A 2006 study showed that drivers talking on cell phones were more involved in rear-end collisions and sped up slower than intoxicated drivers.[28] When talking, people must withdraw their attention from the road in order to formulate responses. Because the brain cannot focus on two sources of input at one time, driving and listening or talking, constantly changing input provided by cell phones distracts the brain and increases the likelihood of accidents.

Supertasker

In 2010, a scientific study found that a small percent of the population appeared to be much better at multitasking than others, and these people were subsequently labeled "supertaskers".[29] In 2014, a follow-up study was found examining the brain activitities of these individuals.[30]

Popular commentary on practical multitasking

Barry Schwartz has noted that, given the media-rich landscape of the Internet era, it is tempting to get into a habit of dwelling in a constant sea of information with too many choices, which has been noted to have a negative effect on human happiness.[31]

Observers of youth in modern society often comment upon the apparently advanced multitasking capabilities of the youngest generations of humans (Generation Y and Generation Z). While it is true that contemporary researchers find that youths in today's world exhibit high levels of multitasking, most experts believe that members of the Net Generation are not any better at multitasking than members of older generations.[32] However, recent studies by Bardhi, Rohm, and Sultan argue that Generation Y is becoming better at media multitasking. This is evidenced by the fact that they are gaining control over deciding which messages they pay attention to or not.[33] Nonetheless, while there is a great deal of evidence showing the negative effects of multitasking on cognitive tasks,[34][35][36][37][38] there is no evidence showing that multitasking has a positive or neutral effect on these tasks.

Many studies,[39][40][41][42][43] literature,[44] articles[45][46][47] and worldwide consulting firms,[48] including recent ones from Louisiana State University psychology Professor Emily Elliott[39] stresses the fact that multitasking of any kind reduces the productivity and/or increases rate of errors, thus generating unnecessary frustrations.

It has been estimated that $650 billion[49] a year is wasted in US businesses due to multitasking.

See also

References

  1. IBM Operating System/360 Concepts and Facilities - Witt, Bernard I. & Lambert, Ward
  2. "You say Multitasking like it's a good thing" by Charles J. Abate, March/April 2009 issue of NEAtoday
  3. Pashler, Harold (1 January 1994). "Dual-task interference in simple tasks: Data and theory.". Psychological Bulletin. 116 (2): 220–244. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.116.2.220. PMID 7972591.
  4. "Is multi-tasking a myth?". BBC News. August 20, 2010.
  5. Hallowell, Edward M.. Crazy Busy: Overstretched, Overbooked, and About to Snap! Strategies for Handling Your Fast-Paced Life. 2007. Ballantine Books. ISBN 0-345-48244-1
  6. Mayer, R. E., & Moreno, R. (2003). Nine ways to reduce cognitive load in multimedia learning. Educational Psychologist, 38(1), 43-52.
  7. Junco, R. & Cotten, S. (2010). Perceived academic effects of instant messaging use. Computers & Education, 56(2), 370-378.
  8. Junco, R. & Cotten, S. (2012). No A 4 U: The relationship between multitasking and academic performance. Computers & Education, 59(2), 505–514.
  9. 1 2 3 Wallis, Claudia (Mar 19, 2006). "The Multitasking Generation" (PDF). Retrieved 2010-04-26.
  10. Lin, Lin (Sep 11, 2008). "Multitasking in Today's Learning Environment:Does Technology Make a Difference? University of North Texas." (PDF). Retrieved 4/26/10. Check date values in: |access-date= (help)
  11. 1 2 3 Rosen, Christine (2008). "The Myth of Multitasking" (PDF). Retrieved 2010-04-26.
  12. Moran, Melanie (2009). "Training Can Improve Multitasking Ability". Retrieved 2010-04-26.
  13. Klingberg, Torkel (2009). The Overflowing Brain: Information Overload and the Limits of Working Memory. Oxford: Oxford UP. pp. 7, 8. ISBN 0-19-537288-3.
  14. "Multitasking Brain Divides And Conquers, To A Point". Retrieved April 23, 2010.
  15. Dean, Jeremy (April 2013). "Are men or women better at multitasking?". PsyBlog: understand your mind.
  16. Morgan, James. "Women 'better at multitasking' than men, study finds". BBC News - science and environment. Retrieved 31 January 2014.
  17. Gray, Richard (17 July 2010). "Scientists prove that women are better at multitasking than men". The Daily Telegraph. Retrieved 31 January 2014.
  18. Ren, Dongning; Zhou, Haotian; Fu, Xiaolan (August 2009). "A deeper look at gender difference in multitasking: gender-specific mechanism of cognitive control". Fifth International Conference on Natural Computation, 2009. ICNC '09. IEEE Xplore - digital library: 13–17. doi:10.1109/ICNC.2009.542.
  19. "Men, not women, are better multitaskers". Cosmos Magazine. 25 October 2012. Retrieved 10 July 2013.
  20. "Brain Connectivity Study Reveals Striking Differences Between Men and Women". 16 April 2014. Retrieved 16 April 2014.
  21. "Sex differences in the structural connectome of the human brain". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 16 April 2014. Retrieved 16 April 2014.
  22. Ren, D.; Zhou, H.; Fu, X. (2009-08-01). "A Deeper Look at Gender Difference in Multitasking: Gender-Specific Mechanism of Cognitive Control". 2009 Fifth International Conference on Natural Computation. 5: 13–17. doi:10.1109/ICNC.2009.542.
  23. Everything Bad Is Good for You: How Today's Popular Culture Is Actually Making Us Smarter by Steven Berlin Johnson, p.61
  24. Continuous Partial Attention
  25. Foehr, Ulla (12 2006). "MEDIA MULTITASKING AMONG AMERICAN YOUTH" (PDF). The Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation. Kaiser Family Foundation. Retrieved 3-8-2010. Check date values in: |access-date=, |date= (help)
  26. Amanda Lenhart; Paul Hitlin; Mary Madden (Jul 27, 2005). "the Pew Internet and American Life Project: Part 1: Basic Demographics of Online Teens and Their Families". Retrieved February 17, 2013.
  27. 1 2 Daniel L. Schacter; Daniel T. Gilbert; Daniel M. Wegner (2011) [2009]. Psychology, 2nd edition. Worth Publishers. p. 132. ISBN 978-1-4292-3719-2.
  28. Strayer, David L.; Drews, Frank A.; Crouch, Dennis J. (Summer 2006). "A Comparison of the Cell Phone Driver and the Drunk Driver". Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society. 48 (2): 381–391. doi:10.1518/001872006777724471. Retrieved 25 July 2014.(subscription required)
  29. "How the Brains of 'Super-Multitaskers' Are Different". Retrieved 2016-09-30.
  30. Fischer, Rico; Plessow, Franziska (2015-01-01). "Efficient multitasking: parallel versus serial processing of multiple tasks". Cognition: 1366. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01366. PMC 4561751Freely accessible. PMID 26441742.
  31. Schwartz, Barry (2004). The Paradox of Choice: Why More Is Less. Ecco. ISBN 0-06-000569-6.
  32. Carrier, L Mark, Cheever, Nancy A, Rosen, Larry D, Benitez, Sandra, & Chang, Jennifer (2009). "Multitasking across generations: Multitasking choices and difficulty ratings in three generations of Americans", Computers in Human Behavior, Volume 25, p483–489.
  33. Bardhi, F.; Rohm, A. J.; Sultan, F. (2010). "Tuning in and tuning out: Media multitasking among young consumers". Journal of Consumer Behaviour. 9 (4): 316–332. doi:10.1002/cb.320.
  34. Koch I.; Lawo V.; Fels J.; Vorländer M. (2011). "Switching in the cocktail party: exploring intentional control of auditory selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology". Human Perception and Performance. 37 (4): 1140–1147. doi:10.1037/a0022189.
  35. Marois R.; Ivanoff J. (2005). "Capacity limits of information processing in the brain". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 9 (6): 296–305. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.04.010.
  36. Strayer D. L.; Drews F. A. (2004). "Profiles in driver distraction: effects of cell phone conversations on younger and older drivers". Human Factors. 46 (4): 640–649. doi:10.1518/hfes.46.4.640.56806.
  37. Tombu, M. N., Asplund, C. L., Dux, P. E., Godwin, D., Martin, J. W., & Marois, R. (2011). A unified attentional bottleneck in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(33).
  38. Wood, N., & Cowan, N. (1995). The cocktail party phenomenon revisited. How frequent are attention shifts to one’s name in an irrelevant auditory channel. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(1), 255–260.
  39. 1 2 Elliott, Emily (18 September 2012). "Louisiana State University psychology professor". ITWorld. Louisiana State University. Retrieved 18 September 2012.
  40. Robert Rogers; Stephen Monsell (1995). "The costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks". Journal of Experimental Psychology. pp. 124, 207–231.
  41. Rubinstein, Joshua S.; Meyer, David E.; Evans, Jeffrey E. (2001). Executive Control of Cognitive Processes in Task Switching. Human Perception and Performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology.
  42. "How Employers Can Make Us Stop Multitasking". Harvard Business Review. Retrieved May 17, 2012.
  43. "Multitasking Gets You There Later". infoQ. June 2010.
  44. Crenshaw, Dave (2008). The myth of multitasking : how doing it all gets nothing done (1st ed.). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. p. 144. ISBN 978-0470372258.
  45. RICHTEL, Matt (April 20, 2011). "Message to Executives: Stop Multitasking". The New York Times Blog.
  46. Cherry, Kendra. "The Cognitive Costs of Multitasking". about.com : Cognitive Psychology.
  47. "Multitasking kills productivity and that's bad for new business".
  48. Derek Dean; Caroline Webb (January 2011). "Recovering from information overload". McKinsey Quarterly. McKinsey.
  49. RICHTEL, Matt (14 June 2008). "Lost in E-Mail, Tech Firms Face Self-Made Beast". The New York Times. Retrieved June 14, 2008.

Further reading

External links

This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the 11/29/2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.