Handedness

This article is about left and right handedness in humans. For other uses, see Handedness (disambiguation).
"Left-hander" redirects here. For other uses, see Left-Hander (disambiguation).
"Southpaw" redirects here. For the boxing stance, see Southpaw stance. For other uses, see Southpaw (disambiguation).

Handedness is a better (faster or more precise) performance or individual preference for use of a hand, known as the dominant hand.[1] Handedness is not a discrete variable (right or left), but a continuous one that can be expressed at levels between strong left and strong right.[2]

There are four types of handedness: left-handedness, right-handedness, mixed-handedness, and ambidexterity.[1] Left-handedness is somewhat more common among men than among women.[3]

Types

Developmental theories of handedness

There are several theories of how handedness develops in individual humans. Occurrences during prenatal development may be important; researchers studied fetuses in utero and determined that handedness in the womb was a very accurate predictor of handedness after birth.[6] In a 2013 study, 39% of infants (6 to 14 months) and 97% of toddlers (18 to 24 months) demonstrated a hand preference.[7]

Division of labor

One common theory, as to how handedness affects the hemispheres, is the brain hemisphere division of labor. Since speaking and handiwork require fine motor skills, its presumption is that it would be more efficient to have one brain hemisphere do both, rather than having it divided up. Since in most people, the left side of the brain controls speaking, right-handedness predominates. This theory also predicts that left-handed people have a reversed brain division of labor.[8]

Verbal processing in right-handed individuals takes place mostly in the left hemisphere, whereas visuospatial processing is mostly done in the opposite hemisphere. Left-handed individuals have a heterogeneous brain organization in which their brain hemisphere is either organized in the same way as right-handers (but with the hemispheres reversed) or even such that both hemispheres are used for verbal processing. When the average is taken across all types of left-handedness, it shows that left-handers are less lateralized.[8]

Genetic factors

Handedness displays a complex inheritance pattern. For example, if both parents of a child are left-handed, there is a 26% chance of that child being left-handed.[9] A large study of twins from 25,732 families by Medland et al. (2006) has indicated that the heritability of handedness is roughly 24%.[10]

To date, two theoretical single gene models have been proposed to explain the patterns of inheritance of handedness, the first by Marian Annett[11] of the University of Leicester and the second by Professor Chris McManus[9] of UCL.

However, the growing weight of evidence from linkage and genome-wide association studies suggests that genetic variance in handedness cannot be explained by a single genetic locus.[12][13][14][15][16][17][18][19] From these studies McManus et al. now conclude that handedness is polygenic and estimate that at least 40 loci contribute to determining this trait.[20]

Brandler et al. performed a genome-wide association study for a measure of relative hand skill and found that genes involved in the determination of left/right asymmetry in the body play a key role in determining handedness.[21] These results suggests the same mechanisms that determine left/right asymmetry in the body (e.g. Nodal signaling and ciliogenesis) also play a role in the development of brain asymmetry (handedness is an outward reflection of brain asymmetry for motor function).

Prenatal hormone exposure

A 2003 study endorsed by the Centers for Disease Control determine that males with in-utero exposure to diethylstilbestrol (a synthetic estrogen-based fertility drug) were more likely to be left-handed over the clinical control group. Diethylstilbestrol animal studies "suggest that estrogen affects the developing brain, including the part that governs sexual behavior and right and left dominance".[22]

Prenatal vestibular asymmetry

Previc, after reviewing a large number of studies, found evidence that the position of the fetus in the final trimester and a baby's subsequent birth position can affect handedness. About two-thirds of fetuses present with their left occiput (back of the head) at birth. This partly explains why prematurity results in a decrease in right-handedness. Previc argues that asymmetric prenatal positioning creates asymmetric stimulation of the vestibular system, which is involved in the development of handedness. In fact, every major disorder in which patients show reduced right-handedness is associated with either vestibular abnormalities or delay,[23] and asymmetry of the vestibular cortex is strongly correlated with the direction of handedness.[24]

Ultrasound

Another theory is that ultrasound may affect the brains of unborn children, causing higher rates of left-handedness in children whose mothers received ultrasounds during pregnancy. Research on this topic suggests there may exist a weak association between ultrasound screening (sonography used to check on the healthy development of the fetus and mother during pregnancy) and non-right-handedness.[25]

Features

Intelligence

In his book Right-Hand, Left-Hand, Chris McManus of University College London argues that the proportion of left-handers is increasing and left-handed people as a group have historically produced an above-average quota of high achievers. He says that left-handers' brains are structured differently (in a way that increases their range of abilities) and the genes that determine left-handedness also govern development of the language centers of the brain.[26]

Writing in Scientific American, McManus states that,

Studies in the U.K., U.S. and Australia have revealed that left-handed people differ from right-handers by only one IQ point, which is not noteworthy ... Left-handers’ brains are structured differently from right-handers’ in ways that can allow them to process language, spatial relations and emotions in more diverse and potentially creative ways. Also, a slightly larger number of left-handers than right-handers are especially gifted in music and math. A study of musicians in professional orchestras found a significantly greater proportion of talented left-handers, even among those who played instruments that seem designed for right-handers, such as violins. Similarly, studies of adolescents who took tests to assess mathematical giftedness found many more left-handers in the population.[27]

According to a 2014 study published in the Harvard Gazette, right-handed people have higher cognitive skills, fewer behavioral and speech problems, less often have learning disabilities, and more often graduate their school.[28]

Early childhood intelligence

Nelson, Campbell, and Michel studied infants and whether developing handedness during infancy correlated with language abilities in toddlers. In the article they assessed 38 infants and followed them through to 12 months and then again once they became toddlers from 18–24 months. What they discovered was that when a child developed a consistent use of its right or left hand during infancy (such as using the right hand to put the pacifier back in, or grasping random objects with the left hand), it was more likely to have superior language skills as a toddler. Children who became lateral later than infancy (i.e., when they were toddlers) showed normal development of language and had typical language scores.[29] The researchers used Bayley scales of infant and toddler development to assess all the subjects.

Health

Lower-birth-weight and complications at birth are positively correlated with left-handness.[28] An excess of non-right-handedness in schizophrenia is demonstrated as an empirical effect.[30] One systematic review concluded: "Left-handers showed no systematic tendency to suffer from disorders of the immune system".[31]

These health problems mean left-handness should be eliminated through natural selection. However, left-handers enjoy an advantage in fighting and sports increasing their likelihood of reproduction.[32]

Income

In a 2006 U.S. study, researchers from Lafayette College and Johns Hopkins University concluded that there was no scientifically significant correlation between handedness and earnings for the general population, but among college-educated people, left-handers earned 10 to 15% more than their right-handed counterparts.[33]

More recently, in a 2014 study published by the National Bureau of Economic Research, Harvard economist Joshua Goodman finds that left-handed people earn 10 to 12 percent less over the course of their lives than right-handed people. Goodman attributes this disparity to higher rates of emotional and behavioral problems in left-handed people.[34]

Left-handedness and sports

See also: Southpaw stance

Interactive sports such as table tennis, badminton, cricket, and tennis have an overrepresentation of left-handedness, while non-interactive sports such as swimming show no overrepresentation. Smaller physical distance between participants increases the overrepresentation. In fencing, about half the participants are left-handed.[35]

The advantage to players in one-on-one sports, such as tennis, boxing, fencing or judo, is that, in a population containing perhaps 10% left-handers and 90% right-handers, the left-hander plays 90% of his or her games against right-handed opponents and is well-practiced at dealing with this asymmetry. Right-handers play 90% of their games against other right-handers. Thus, when confronted with left-handers, they are less practiced (see Rafael Nadal). When two left-handers compete against each other, they are both likely to be at the same level of practice as when right-handers play other right-handers. This explains why a disproportionately high number of left-handers are found in sports in which direct one-on-one action predominates.

Other, sports-specific factors may increase or decrease the advantage left-handers usually hold in one-on-one situations:

In sports in which one competitor's performance does not affect another's (except indirectly through subjectively perceived psychological pressure), a particular hand preference confers little or no advantage. Golf and miniature golf feature occasional situations when obstacles on one side of the ball but not the other interfere with the stance or swing of a right- or left-handed player but not the other's. Even so, the "favoritism" on any given course is probably minimal, especially at high levels of play: a layperson such as the owner of a small miniature golf business may, when placing obstacles, assess the results from only the perspective of his or her handedness, such that more courses would be made difficult for right-handers than for left-handers. However, a thoughtful designer—especially a professional in the field—is likely to ensure game balance by adding handedness-specific obstacles in equal numbers and in places of similar tactical importance.

Gender Ratio and Handedness

Left-handedness is more common among men than women. It is estimated that left-handedness is around 20% more common among men than women. [3]

Sexuality and gender identity

A number of studies examining the relationship between handedness and sexual orientation have reported that a disproportionate minority of homosexual people exhibit non-right-handedness,[42] though finds are mixed.[43][44][45]

A 2001 study also found that children who were born male but have non-traditional gender identities were more than twice as likely to be left-handed than a clinical control group (19.5% vs. 8.3%, respectively).[46]

Paraphilias (atypical sexual interests) have also been linked to higher rates of non-right-handedness. A 2008 study analyzing the sexual fantasies of 200 males found "elevated paraphilic interests were correlated with elevated non-right handedness."[47] Greater rates of non-right-handedness has also been documented among individuals who have pedophilia.[48][49][50][51]

A 2014 study attempting to analyze the biological markers of asexuality asserts that non-sexual men and women were 2.4 and 2.5 times, respectively, more likely to be non-right-handed than their heterosexual counterparts.[52]

Social stigma and repression of left-handedness

Many tools and procedures are designed to facilitate use by right-handed people, often without even realizing difficulties placed on the left-handed. John W. Santrock has written, "For centuries, left-handers have suffered unfair discrimination in a world designed for right-handers."[53]

McManus noted that, beginning at the time of the Industrial Revolution, workers needed to operate complex machines that were almost certainly designed with right-handers in mind. This would have made left-handers more visible and at the same time appear less capable and more clumsy. During this era, children were taught to write with a dip pen. While a right-hander could smoothly drag the pen across paper from left to right, a dip pen could not easily be pushed across by the left hand without digging into the paper and making blots and stains.[54]

Moreover, apart from inconvenience, left-handed people have been considered unlucky or even malicious for their difference by the right-handed majority. In many European languages, including English, the word for the direction "right" also means "correct" or "proper". Throughout history, being left-handed was considered negative. The Latin adjective sinister means "left" as well as "unlucky", and this double meaning survives in European derivatives of Latin, including the English word "sinister" (only when referring to the bearer's left of a coat of arms).

There are many negative connotations associated with the phrase "left-handed": clumsy, awkward, unlucky, insincere, sinister, malicious, and so on. A "left-handed compliment" is considered one that is unflattering or dismissive in meaning. In French, gauche means both "left" and "awkward" or "clumsy", while droit(e) (cognate to English direct and related to "adroit") means both "right" and "straight", as well as "law" and the legal sense of "right". The name "Dexter" derives from the Latin for "right", as does the word "dexterity" meaning manual skill. As these are all very old words, they would tend to support theories indicating that the predominance of right-handedness is an extremely old phenomenon.

Black magic is sometimes referred to as the "left-hand path".

Until very recently in Taiwan (and still in Mainland China), left-handed people were strongly encouraged to switch to being right-handed, or at least switch to writing with the right hand. Due to the importance of stroke order, developed for the comfortable use of right-handed people, it is considered more difficult to write legible Chinese characters with the left hand than it is to write Latin letters, though difficulty is subjective and depends on the writer.[55] Because writing when moving one's hand away from its side towards the other side of the body can cause smudging if the outward side of the hand is allowed to drag across the writing, writing in the Latin alphabet might possibly be less feasible with the left hand than the right under certain circumstances. Conversely, right-to-left alphabets, such as the Arabic and Hebrew, are generally considered easier to write with the left hand in general. Depending on the position and inclination of the writing paper, and the writing method, the left-handed writer can write as neatly and efficiently or as messily and slowly as right-handed writers. Usually the left-handed child needs to be taught how to write correctly with the left hand, since discovering a comfortable left-handed writing method on one's own may not be straightforward.[56][57]

International Left-Handers Day

International Left-Handers Day is held annually every August 13.[58] It was founded by the Left-Handers Club in 1992, with the club itself having been founded in 1990.[58] International Left-Handers Day is, according to the club, "an annual event when left-handers everywhere can celebrate their sinistrality [meaning left-handedness] and increase public awareness of the advantages and disadvantages of being left-handed."[58] It celebrates their uniqueness and differences, who are from seven to ten percent of the world's population. Thousands of left-handed people in today's society have to adapt to use right handed tools and objects. Again according to the club, "in the U.K. alone there were over 20 regional events to mark the day in 2001- including left-v-right sports matches, a left-handed tea party, pubs using left-handed corkscrews where patrons drank and played pub games with the left hand only, and nationwide 'Lefty Zones' where left-handers' creativity, adaptability and sporting prowess were celebrated, whilst right-handers were encouraged to try out everyday left-handed objects to see just how awkward it can feel using the wrong equipment!"[58]

In animals

Kangaroos and other macropod marsupials have a left-hand preference for everyday tasks in the wild. ‘True’ handedness is unexpected in marsupials because, unlike placental mammals, they lack a corpus callosum. Left-handedness was particularly apparent in the red kangaroo (Macropus rufus) and the eastern gray kangaroo (Macropus giganteus). Red-necked (Bennett’s) wallabies (Macropus rufogriseus) preferentially use their left hand for behaviours that involve fine manipulation, but the right for behaviours that require more physical strength. There was less evidence for handedness in arboreal species.[59] Studies of dogs, horses, and domestic cats have shown that females of those species tend to be right handed, while males are lefties.[60]

See also

In general
Handedness

Sources

  1. 1 2 3 Holder, M.K. What does Handedness have to do with Brain Lateralization (and who cares?). Retrieved 11 August 2012.
  2. 1 2 Annett, Marian (2002). Handedness and Brain Asymmetry. Psychology Press.
  3. 1 2 Papadatou-Pastou, M; Martin, M; Munafò, MR; Jones, GV (September 2008). "Sex differences in left-handedness: a meta-analysis of 144 studies". Psychological Bulletin. 134 (5): 677–99. doi:10.1037/a0012814. PMID 18729568.
  4. Holder, M. K. (1997). "Why are more people right-handed?". Sciam.com. Scientific American Inc. Retrieved 2008-04-14.
  5. Hardyck C, Petrinovich LF (1977). "Left-handedness". Psychol Bull. 84 (3): 385–404. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.84.3.385. PMID 859955.
  6. "Prenatal thumb sucking is related to postnatal handedness".
  7. Nelson EL, Campbell JM, Michel GF (2013). "Unimanual to bimanual: Tracking the development of handedness from 6 to 24 months". Infant Behavior and Development. 36 (2): 181–188. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.01.009. PMC 3615031Freely accessible. PMID 23454419.
  8. 1 2 Banich, Marie (1997). Neuropsychology: The Neural Bases of Mental Function.
  9. 1 2 McManus, Chris (2003). Right Hand, Left Hand. Phoenix Paperbacks. ISBN 0753813556.
  10. "Genetic influences on handedness: Data from 25,732 Australian and Dutch twin families".
  11. Annett, M. (2009). "The genetic basis of lateralization". In Sommer, Iris E. C.; Kahn, René S. Language lateralization and psychosis. pp. 73–86. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511576744.006. ISBN 9780511576744.
  12. Francks, C; DeLisi, LE; Fisher, SE; Laval, SH; Rue, JE; Stein, JF; Monaco, AP (February 2003). "Confirmatory evidence for linkage of relative hand skill to 2p12-q11". American Journal of Human Genetics. 72 (2): 499–502. doi:10.1086/367548. PMC 379245Freely accessible. PMID 12596796.
  13. Francks C, Maegawa S, Laurén J, Abrahams BS, Velayos-Baeza A, Medland SE, Colella S, Groszer M, McAuley EZ, Caffrey TM, Timmusk T, Pruunsild P, Koppel I, Lind PA, Matsumoto-Itaba N, Nicod J, Xiong L, Joober R, Enard W, Krinsky B, Nanba E, Richardson AJ, Riley BP, Martin NG, Strittmatter SM, Möller HJ, Rujescu D, St Clair D, Muglia P, Roos JL, Fisher SE, Wade-Martins R, Rouleau GA, Stein JF, Karayiorgou M, Geschwind DH, Ragoussis J, Kendler KS, Airaksinen MS, Oshimura M, DeLisi LE, Monaco, AP (December 2007). "LRRTM1 on chromosome 2p12 is a maternally suppressed gene that is associated paternally with handedness and schizophrenia". Molecular Psychiatry. 12 (12): 1129–39, 1057. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4002053. PMC 2990633Freely accessible. PMID 17667961.
  14. Van Agtmael, T; Forrest, SM; Williamson, R (October 2002). "Parametric and non-parametric linkage analysis of several candidate regions for genes for human handedness". European Journal of Human Genetics. 10 (10): 623–30. doi:10.1038/sj.ejhg.5200851. PMID 12357333.
  15. Warren, Diane M.; Stern, Michael; Duggirala, Ravindranath; Dyer, Thomas D.; Almasy, Laura (1 November 2006). "Heritability and linkage analysis of hand, foot, and eye preference in Mexican Americans". Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition. 11 (6): 508–524. doi:10.1080/13576500600761056.
  16. Laval SH, Dann JC, Butler RJ, Loftus J, Rue J, Leask SJ, Bass N, Comazzi M, Vita A, Nanko S, Shaw S, Peterson P, Shields G, Smith AB, Stewart J, DeLisi LE, Crow TJ (September 7, 1998). "Evidence for linkage to psychosis and cerebral asymmetry (relative hand skill) on the X chromosome". American Journal of Medical Genetics. 81 (5): 420–7. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1096-8628(19980907)81:5<420::AID-AJMG11>3.0.CO;2-E. PMID 9754628.
  17. Armour, JA; Davison, A; McManus, IC (Sep 25, 2013). "Genome-wide association study of handedness excludes simple genetic models.". Heredity. 112 (3): 221–5. doi:10.1038/hdy.2013.93. PMID 24065183.
  18. Eriksson, N; Macpherson, JM; Tung, JY; Hon, LS; Naughton, B; Saxonov, S; Avey, L; Wojcicki, A; Pe'er, I; Mountain, J (Jun 24, 2010). "Web-based, participant-driven studies yield novel genetic associations for common traits.". PLOS Genetics. 6 (6): e1000993. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1000993. PMC 2891811Freely accessible. PMID 20585627.
  19. Scerri, TS; Brandler, WM; Paracchini, S; Morris, AP; Ring, SM; Richardson, AJ; Talcott, JB; Stein, J; Monaco, AP (Feb 1, 2011). "PCSK6 is associated with handedness in individuals with dyslexia.". Human Molecular Genetics. 20 (3): 608–14. doi:10.1093/hmg/ddq475. PMC 3016905Freely accessible. PMID 21051773.
  20. McManus, IC; Davison, A; Armour, JA (June 2013). "Multilocus genetic models of handedness closely resemble single-locus models in explaining family data and are compatible with genome-wide association studies.". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1288 (1): 48–58. doi:10.1111/nyas.12102. PMID 23631511.
  21. Brandler WM, Morris AP, Evans DM, Scerri TS, Kemp JP, Timpson NJ, St Pourcain B, Smith GD, Ring SM, Stein J, Monaco AP, Talcott JB, Fisher SE, Webber C, Paracchini S (September 2013). "Common variants in left/right asymmetry genes and pathways are associated with relative hand skill.". PLOS Genetics. 9 (9): e1003751. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1003751. PMC 3772043Freely accessible. PMID 24068947.
  22. Titus-Ernstoff, L. (2003). "Psychosexual Characteristics of Men and Women Exposed Prenatally to Diethylstilbestrol". CDC.
  23. Nonright-handedness, central nervous system and related pathology, and its lateralization: A reformulation and synthesis.
  24. Dieterich, M.; Bense, S.; Lutz, S.; Drzezga, A.; Stephan, T.; Bartenstein, P.; Brandt, T. (2003). "Dominance for vestibular cortical function in the non-dominant hemisphere". Cerebral Cortex. 13 (9): 994–1007. doi:10.1093/cercor/13.9.994. PMID 12902399.
  25. Salvesen, K. Å. (1 September 2011). "Ultrasound in pregnancy and non-right handedness: meta-analysis of randomized trials". Ultrasound in Obstetrics & Gynecology. 38 (3): 267–271. doi:10.1002/uog.9055.
  26. Right-Hand, Left-Hand official website Accessed June 2006.
  27. Chris McManus. April 14, 2012. Is It True That Left-Handed People Are Smarter Than Right-Handed People? Scientific American. Retrieved: 26 May 2013.
  28. 1 2 "A lefty's lament". Harvard Gazette.
  29. Nelson, E.L., J.M. Campbell, and G.F. Michel, Early handedness in infancy predicts language ability in toddlers. Developmental Psychology, 2014. 50(3): p. 809-814.
  30. Hirnstein, Marco; Hugdahl, Kenneth (October 2014). "Excess of non-right-handedness in schizophrenia: meta-analysis of gender effects and potential biases in handedness assessment" (PDF). The British Journal of Psychiatry. 205 (4): 260–267. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.113.137349. Retrieved 2016-05-09.
  31. "Three Myths and Three Facts About Left-Handers". Psychology Today.
  32. Knight, Will (8 December 2004). "Left-handers win in hand-to-hand combat". New Scientist. Retrieved 27 October 2016.
  33. Waldfogel, Joel (August 16, 2006). "Sinister and Rich: The evidence that lefties earn more". Slate.
  34. Goodman, Joshua (Fall 2014). "The Wages of Sinistrality: Handedness, Brain Structure, and Human Capital Accumulation" (PDF). 28 (4). Journal of Economic Perspective: 193–212. doi:10.1257/jep.28.4.193.
  35. Diana Widermann, Robert A. Barton, and Russel A. Hill. Evolutionary perspectives on sport and competition. In Roberts, S. C. (2011). Roberts, S. Craig, ed. "Applied Evolutionary Psychology". Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001. ISBN 9780199586073.
  36. "2012 Major League Baseball Batting Splits". Baseball Reference. Retrieved 6 March 2013.
  37. "The advantage of batting left-handed". hardballtimes.com. Retrieved 7 March 2013.
  38. "Career Leaders & Records for Batting Average". Baseball Reference. Retrieved 6 March 2013.
  39. 1 2 "First Base Pickoffs for Lefty Pitchers". isport.com. Retrieved 7 March 2013.
  40. Miller, Stuart (29 March 2009). "The Decline of Left-Handed First Basemen". nytimes.com. Retrieved 7 March 2013.
  41. 1 2 3 4 "Left Handed Catchers". The Encyclopedia of Baseball Catchers. Retrieved 7 March 2013.
  42. Lalumière ML, Blanchard R, Zucker KJ (2000). "Sexual orientation and handedness in men and women: a meta-analysis". Psychological Bulletin. 126 (4): 575–592. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.4.575. PMID 10900997.
  43. Mustanski, B. S.; Bailey, J. M.; Kaspar, S. (2002). "Dermatoglyphics, handedness, sex, and sexual orientation". Archives of Sexual Behavior. 31 (1): 113–122. doi:10.1023/A:1014039403752. PMID 11910784.
  44. Terrance J. Williams; Michelle E. Pepitone; Scott E. Christensen; Bradley M. Cooke; Andrew D. Huberman; Nicholas J. Breedlove; Tessa J. Breedlove; Cynthia L. Jordan; S. Marc Breedlove (2000). "Finger-length ratios and sexual orientation" (PDF). Nature. 404 (6777): 455–456. doi:10.1038/35006555. PMID 10761903.
  45. Gene Schwartz; et al. (2010). "Biodemographic and Physical Correlates of Sexual Orientation in Men". Arch Sex Behav. 39: 93–109. doi:10.1007/s10508-009-9499-1.
  46. Zucker KJ, Beaulieu N, Bradley SJ, Grimshaw GM, Wilcox A (2001). "Handedness in boys with gender identity disorder". J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 42 (6): 767–776. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00773. PMID 11583249.
  47. Rahman Q, Symeonides DJ, Q.; Symeonides, D. J. (February 2007). "Neurodevelopmental Correlates of Paraphilic Sexual Interests in Men". Archives of Sexual Behavior. 37 (1): 166–172. doi:10.1007/s10508-007-9255-3. PMID 18074220.
  48. Blanchard R, Kolla NJ, Cantor JM, Klassen PE, Dickey R, Kuban ME, Blak T (2007). "IQ, handedness, and pedophilia in adult male patients stratified by referral source.". Sex Abuse. 19: 285–309. doi:10.1007/s11194-007-9049-0. PMID 17634757.
  49. Cantor JM, Klassen PE, Dickey R, Christensen BK, Kuban ME, Blak T, Williams NS, Blanchard R (2005). "Handedness in pedophilia and hebephilia". Archives of Sexual Behavior. 34 (4): 447–459. doi:10.1007/s10508-005-4344-7. PMID 16010467.
  50. Bogaert AF (2001). "Handedness, criminality, and sexual offending". Neuropsychologia. 39 (5): 465–469. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00134-2. PMID 11254928.
  51. Fiona Dyshniku; Michelle E. Murray; Rachel L. Fazio; Amy D. Lykins; James M. Cantor (2015). "Minor Physical Anomalies as a Window into the Prenatal Origins of Pedophilia". Archives of Sexual Behavior. 44: 2151–2159. doi:10.1007/s10508-015-0564-7.
  52. Yule MA, Brotto LA, Gorzalka BB. "Biological markers of asexuality: Handedness, birth order, and finger length ratios in self-identified asexual men and women.". Arch Sex Behav. Retrieved 2014. Check date values in: |access-date= (help)
  53. Santrock, John W. (2008). Motor, Sensory, and Perceptual Development. Mike Ryan [Ed.], A Topical Approach to Life-Span Development(pgs.172–205). Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill Higher Education.
  54. McManus, IC. The history and geography of human handedness. In: Language Lateralization and Psychosis, Iris E. C. Sommer and René S. Kahn. eds. Cambridge Univ. Press, 2009.
  55. "A question of the left being right – and normal". China Daily. February 22, 2008. Archived from the original on January 24, 2011. Retrieved 2011-01-19.
  56. Erste deutsche Beratungs- und Informationsstelle für Linkshänder und umge-schulte Linkshänder. lefthander-consulting.org. Retrieved 21 September 2013
  57. Handedness Research Institute | Teaching Left-Handers to Write. handedness.org. Retrieved 21 September 2013
  58. 1 2 3 4 Left-Handers' Day August 13th: Celebrate your right to be left-handed. lefthandersday.com. Retrieved 12 August 2013
  59. "All kangaroos are lefties, scientists say". Sci-News.com. June 18, 2015. Retrieved June 19, 2015.
  60. "Is your cat left or right pawed?". New Scientist. Retrieved 6 November 2016.

External links

Look up handedness in Wiktionary, the free dictionary.
Wikimedia Commons has media related to Left-handedness.
This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the 12/2/2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.