Flapping

For other uses, see Flap (disambiguation).

Flapping or tapping, also known as alveolar flapping or intervocalic flapping, is a phonological process found in many dialects of English, especially North American English, Australian English and New Zealand English, by which the consonants /t/ and /d/ may be pronounced as a flap (tap) in certain positions, particularly between vowels (intervocalic position). In some cases, the effect is perceived by some listeners as the replacement of a /t/ sound with a /d/ sound; for example, the word butter pronounced with flapping may be heard as "budder".[1] In fact, both /t/ and /d/ are replaced in such positions by an alveolar flap (or tap; IPA symbol [ɾ]), a sound produced by briefly tapping the alveolar ridge with the tongue. Also, in similar positions, the combination /nt/ may be pronounced with a nasalized flap or just [n] so winter may sound similar or identical to winner.

Terminology

The terms flap and tap are often used synonymously, although some authors make a distinction between them. According to Heinz Giegerich, a flap involves a rapid movement of the tongue tip from a retracted vertical position to a (more or less) horizontal position, during which the tongue tip brushes the alveolar ridge, while a tap involves a rapid upward and downward movement of the tongue tip, the upward movement being voluntary and the lowering involuntary. On this view, the sound referred to here is the alveolar tap, rather than the flap, even though the term "flapping" is ingrained in much of the phonological literature.[2]

Even though taps and flaps are sometimes distinguished phonetically, no languages are known to contrast them, and they are assigned only one set of symbols in the International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA). The symbol for the alveolar tap (flap) discussed here is ɾ.

Distribution

Flapping/tapping may occur when /t/ or /d/ occurs between two vowels, as in butter, writing, wedding, loader. However, it does not occur, in most dialects, if the /t/ or /d/ immediately precedes a stressed vowel, as in attack [əˈtʰæk], but they can tap across a word boundary, as in got over [ɡɑɾˈoʊvɚ], and when a word boundary is embedded within a word, as in buttinsky [bʌɾˈɪnski]. Australian English also taps word-internally before a stressed vowel in words like fourteen.

The cluster /nt/ can also be tapped and be pronounced with a nasalized tap (IPA symbol ɾ̃). In quick speech this may become simply [n] so words like winner and winter can become homophonous. Tapping does not occur, for most speakers, in words like carpenter and ninety, which instead surface with [d].

It is also reported that tapping may occur after r, as in barter and sometimes after l, as in faculty (but not immediately after the stress: alter → [ɔːɫtəɹ], not [*ɔːɫɾəɹ]).

Homophony

Flapping/tapping is a specific type of lenition, specifically intervocalic weakening. It leads to the neutralization of the distinction between /t/ and /d/ in appropriate environments; a partial merger of the two phonemes. For speakers with the merger, the following utterances sound the same or almost the same (click "show" to display the table):

For most speakers, the merger does not occur when an intervocalic /t/ or /d/ is followed by a syllabic n so written and ridden remain distinct. A few number of speakers (including pockets in the Boston area) lack the rule that glottalizes t and d before syllabic n and so flap/tap /t/ and /d/ in that environment. Pairs like potent-impotent, with the former having a preglottalized unreleased t or a glottal stop (but not a flap/tap) and the latter having either an aspirated t or a flap/tap, suggest that the level of stress on the preceding vowel may play a role in the applicability of glottalization and flapping/tapping before syllabic n. Some speakers in the Pacific Northwest turn /t/ into a flap but not /d/ so writer and rider remain distinct although the i is pronounced the same in both words.

In accents characterized by Canadian raising, such words as riding and writing, both of which have an alveolar flap, continue to be distinguished by the preceding vowel. The consonant distinction is neutralized, the underlying voice distinction continues to select the allophone of the /aɪ/ phoneme preceding it. Thus, for many North Americans, riding is [ɹaɪɾɪŋ] while writing is [ɹɐɪɾɪŋ]. Vowel duration may also be different, with a longer vowel before tap realizations of /d/ than before tap realizations of /t/. At the phonetic level, the contrast between /t/ and /d/ may be maintained by the non-local cues, but as the cues are quite subtle, they may not be acquired/perceived by others. A merger of /t, d/ can thus be said to have occurred then.

Other languages

A similar process also occurs in other languages, such as Western Apache (and other Southern Athabaskan languages). In Western Apache, intervocalic /t/ similarly is realized as [ɾ] in intervocalic position. The process occurs even between words. However, tapping is blocked when /t/ is the initial consonant of a stem so tapping occurs only when /t/ is stem-internal or in a prefix. Unlike English, tapping is not affected by suprasegmentals (stress or tone). Another important example is Tagalog, in this case involving /d/ vs. [ɾ].

See also

References

  1. See for example: Kirsten Fox, English Language Exam Guide, Insight Publications, 2011, p. 158.
  2. Giegerich, Heinz J. (1992). English Phonology, pp. 225, 241. Cambridge University Press.

External links

This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the 9/14/2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.