Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex

Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex

An illustration of brain's prefrontal region
Details
Identifiers
Latin Cortex praefrontalis dorsolateralis
NeuroLex ID Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex

Anatomical terms of neuroanatomy

The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC or DL-PFC) is an area in the prefrontal cortex of the brain of humans and non-human primates. It is one of the most recently evolved parts of the human brain that undergoes an extremely prolonged period of maturation that lasts until adulthood.[1] DLPFC is not an anatomical structure, but rather a functional one. This region lies in the middle frontal gyrus of humans (i.e., lateral part of Brodmann's area (BA) 9 and 46[2] and in macaque monkeys, this region is around the principal sulcus (i.e., in Walker's area 46[3]).[4][5] Other sources consider that DLPFC is attributed anatomically to BA 9 and 46[6] and BA 8, 9 and 10.[1]
DLPFC is connected to the orbitofrontal cortex, and to a variety of brain areas, which include the thalamus, parts of the basal ganglia (specifically, the dorsal caudate nucleus), the hippocampus, and primary and secondary association areas of neocortex, including posterior temporal, parietal, and occipital areas.[7] Also, DLPFC is the end point for the dorsal pathway (stream) that tells the brain how to interact with the stimuli. On the other hand, the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (located more inferior/ventral to DLPFC) is the end point of the ventral pathway (stream) that brings information about the stimuli’s characteristics.[8]
An important function of the DLPFC is the executive functions, such as working memory, cognitive flexibility, planning, inhibition, and abstract reasoning.[9] However, DLPFC is not exclusively responsible for the executive functions. All complex mental activity requires the additional cortical and subcortical circuits with which the DLPFC is connected.[10] The DLPFC is also the highest cortical area that is involved in motor planning, organization and regulation.[10]

Structure

As DLPFC is composed of spatial selective neurons, it has a neural circuitry that encompasses the entire range of sub-functions necessary to carry out an integrated response such as: sensory input, retention in short-term memory, and motor signaling.[11] Historically dorsolateral prefrontal cortex was defined by its connection to: superior temporal cortex, posterior parietal cortex, anterior and posterior cingulate, premotor cortex, retrosplenial cortex, and neocerebellum,[1] as these connections allow DLPFC to regulate the activity of those regions and as well receive information from, and be regulated by those regions.[1]

Function

Primary functions

DLPFC is known for its involvement in the executive functions, which is an umbrella term for the management of cognitive processes,[12] including working memory, cognitive flexibility,[13] and planning.[14] A couple of tasks have been very prominent in the research on the DLPFC, such as the A-not-B task, the delayed response task and object retrieval tasks.[1] The behavioral task that is most strongly linked to DLPFC is the combined A-not-B/delayed response task, in which the subject has to find a hidden object after a certain delay. This task requires holding the information in mind (working memory) which is believed to be one of the functions of DLPFC.[1] The importance of DLPFC for working memory was strengthened by studies with adult macaques. Lesions that destroyed DLPFC disrupted the macaques’ performance of the A-not-B/delayed response task, whereas lesions to other brain parts did not impair their performance on this task.[1]

DLPFC is not required for the memory of a single item. Thus, damage to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex does not impair recognition memory.[15] Nevertheless, if two items must be compared from memory, the involvement of DLPFC is required. People with damaged DLPFC are not able to identify a picture they had seen, after some time, when given the opportunity to choose from two pictures.[15] Moreover, these subjects also failed in Wisconsin Card-Sorting Test as they lose track of the currently correct rule and persistently organize their cards in the previously correct rule.[16] In addition, as DLPFC deals with waking thought and reality testing, it is not active when one is asleep.[16] Likewise, DLPFC is most frequently related to the dysfunction of drive, attention and motivation.[17] Patients with minor DLPFC damage display disinterest in their surroundings and are deprived of spontaneity in language as well as behavior.[17] Patients may also be less alert than normal to people and events they know.[17] Damage to this region in a person also leads to the lack of motivation to do things for themselves and/or for others.[17]

Decision making

The DLPFC is involved in both risky and moral decision making; when individuals have to make moral decisions like how to distribute limited resources, the DLPFC is activated.[18] This region is also active when costs and benefits of alternative choices are of interest.[19] Similarly, when options for choosing alternatives are present, the DLPFC evokes a preference towards the most equitable option and suppresses the temptation to maximize personal gain.[20]

Working memory

Working memory is the system that actively holds multiple pieces of transitory information in the mind, where they can be manipulated. The DLPFC is important for working memory;[21] reduced activity in this area correlates to poor performance on working memory tasks.[22] However, other areas of the brain are involved in working memory as well.[23]


There is an ongoing discussion if the DLPFC is specialized in a certain type of working memory, namely computational mechanisms for monitoring and manipulating items, or if it has a certain content, namely visuospatial information, which makes it possible to mentally represent coordinates within the spatial domain.[21]

There have also been some suggestions that the function of the DLPFC in verbal and spatial working memory is lateralised into the left and right hemisphere, respectively. Smith, Jonides and Koeppe (1996)[24] observed a lateralisation of DLPFC activations during verbal and visual working memory. Verbal working memory tasks mainly activated the left DLPFC and visual working memory tasks mainly activated the right DLPFC. Murphy et al. (1998)[25] also found that verbal working memory tasks activated the right and left DLPFC, whereas spatial working memory tasks predominantly activated the left DLPFC. Reuter-Lorenz et al. (2000)[26] found that activations of the DLPFC showed prominent lateralisation of verbal and spatial WM in young adults, whereas in older adults this lateralisation was less noticeable. It was proposed that this reduction in lateralisation could be due to recruitment of neurones from the opposite hemisphere to compensate for neuronal decline with ageing.

Secondary functions

The DLPFC may also be involved in the act of deception and lying,[27] which is thought to inhibit normal tendency to truth telling. Research also suggests that using TMS on the DLPFC can impede a person's ability to lie or to tell the truth.[28]

Additionally, supporting evidence suggests that the DLPFC may also play a role in conflict-induced behavioral adjustment, for instance when an individual decides what to do when faced with conflicting rules.[29] One way in which this has been tested is through the Stroop test,[30] in which subjects are shown a name of a color printed in colored ink and then are asked to name the color of the ink as fast as possible. Conflict arises when the color of the ink does not match the name of the printed color. During this experiment, tracking of the subjects’ brain activity showed a noticeable activity within the DLPFC.[30] The activation of the DLPFC correlated with the behavioral performance, which suggests that this region maintains the high demands of the task to resolve conflict, and thus in theory plays a role in taking control.[30]

DLPFC may also be associated with human intelligence. However, even when correlations are found between the DLPFC and human intelligence, that does not mean that all human intelligence is a function of the DLPFC. In other words, this region may be attributed to general intelligence on a broader scale as well as very specific roles, but not all roles. For example, using imaging studies like PET and fMRI indicate DLPFC involvement in deductive, syllogistic reasoning.[31] Specifically, when involved in activities that require syllogistic reasoning, left DLPFC areas are especially and consistently active.[31]

The DLPFC may also be involved in threat-induced anxiety.[32] In one experiment, participants were asked to rate themselves as behaviorally inhibited or not. Those who rated themselves as behaviorally inhibited, moreover, showed greater tonic (resting) activity in the right-posterior DLPFC.[32] Such activity is able to be seen through Electroencephalogram (EEG) recordings. Individuals who are behaviorally inhibited are more likely to experience feelings of stress and anxiety when faced with a particularly threatening situation.[32] In one theory, anxiety susceptibility may increase as a result of present vigilance. Evidence for this theory includes neuroimaging studies that demonstrate DLPFC activity when an individual experiences vigilance.[32] More specifically, it is theorized that threat-induced anxiety may also be connected to deficits in resolving problems, which leads to uncertainty.[32] When an individual experiences uncertainty, there is increased activity in the DLPFC. In other words, such activity can be traced back to threat-induced anxiety.

Social cognition

Among the prefrontal lobes, the DLPFC seems to be the one that has the least direct influence on social behavior, yet it does seem to give clarity and organization to social cognition.[9] The DLPFC seems to contribute to social functions through the operation of its main speciality the executive functions, for instance when handling complex social situations.[9] Social areas in which the role of the DLPFC is investigated are, amongst others, social perspective taking[7] and inferring the intentions of other people,[7] or theory of mind;[9] the suppression of selfish behavior,[7][33] and commitment in a relationship.[34]

Relation to neurotransmitters

As DLPFC undergoes long maturational changes, one change that has been attributed to DLPFC for making early cognitive advances is the increasing level of neurotransmitter dopamine in DLPFC.[1] Studies where the adult macaques' Dopamine receptors were blocked, it was seen that the adult macaques had deficit in the A-not-B task, as if the whole DFPLC was taken out altogether. Similar situation was seen when the macaques were injected with MPTP injection as it reduces the level of Dopamine in the DLPFC.[1] Even though, there have been no physiological studies about involvement of cholinergic actions in sub-cortical areas, behavioral studies indicate that neurotransmitter acetylcholine is essential for working memory function of the DLPFC.[35]

Clinical significance

Schizophrenia

Schizophrenia may be partially attributed to a lack in activity in the frontal lobe.[16] The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is especially under active when a person suffers from chronic schizophrenia. Schizophrenia is also related to lack of dopamine neurotransmitter in the frontal lobe.[16] The DLPFC dysfunctions are unique among the schizophrenia patients as those that are diagnosed with depression do not tend to have the same abnormal activation in the DLPFC during working-memory related tasks.[22] Working Memory is dependent upon the DLPFC’S stability and functionally, thus reduced activation of it causes for schizophrenic patients to perform poorly on tasks involving working memory. The poor performance contributes to the added capacity limitations in working memory that is greater than the limits on normal patients.[36] The cognitive processes that deal heavily with the DLPFC, such as memory, attention, and higher order processing, are the functions that once distorted contribute to the factors of the illness.[22]

Depression

Along with regions of the brains such as the limbic system, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex deals heavily with major depressive disorder (MDD). The DLPFC may contribute to depression due to being involved with the disorder on an emotional level during the suppression stage.[37] While working memory tasks seem to activate the DLPFC normally,[38] its decreased grey matter volume correlates to its decreased activity. The DLPFC may also have ties to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in their functions with depression.[37] This can be attributed to how the DLPFC’s cognitive functions can also involve emotions, and the VMPFC’s emotional effects can also involve self-awareness or self-reflection. Damage or lesion to the DLPFC can also lead to increased expression of depression symptoms.

Stress

Exposure to severe stress may also be linked to damage in the DLPFC.[39] More specifically, acute stress has a negative impact on the higher cognitive function known as working memory (WM), which is also traced to be a function of the DLPFC.[39] In an experiment, researchers used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to record the neural activity in healthy individuals who participated in tasks while in a stressful environment.[39] When stress successfully impacted the subjects, their neural activity showed reduced working memory related activity in the DLPFC.[39] These findings not only demonstrate the importance of the DLPFC region in relation to stress, but they also suggest that the DLPFC may play a role in other psychiatric disorders. In patients with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), for example, daily sessions of right dorsolateral prefrontal repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) at a frequency of 10 Hz resulted in more effective therapeutic stimulation.[40]

Substance abuse

Substance abuse of drugs, or substance use disorder (SUD), has links to the diminished executive functions of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.[41] Those who abuse drugs have been shown to engage in increased risky behavior, possibly correlating with a dysfunction of the DLPFC. The executive controlling functions of the DLPFC in individuals who display drug abuse may have a connection that is lessen from risk factoring areas such as the anterior cingulate cortex and insula.[41] This weakened connection is even shown in healthy subjects, such as a patient who continued to make risky decisions with a disconnect between their DLPFC and insula. Lesions of the DLPFC may result in irresponsibility and freedom from inhibitions,[42] and the abuse of drugs can invoke the same response of willingness or inspiration to engage in daring activity.

Alcohol

Alcohol can create an effect on the functionality of the prefrontal cortex and can contribute to the regulation of alcoholism.[43] As the ACC works to inhibit any inappropriate behaviors through processing information to the executive network of the DLPFC,[43] as noted before this disruption in communication can lead to these actions being made. In a task known as Cambridge risk task, SUD participants have been shown to have a lower activation of their DLPFC. Specifically in a test related to alcoholism, a task called the Wheel of Fortune (WOF) had adolescents with a family history of alcoholism present lower DLPFC activation.[41] Adolescents that have had no family members with a history of alcoholism did not exhibit the same decrease of activity.

See also

This article uses anatomical terminology; for an overview, see Anatomical terminology.

References

  1. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Luciana, ed. by Charles A. Nelson; Monica (2001). Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience. Cambridge, Mass. [u.a.]: MIT Press. ISBN 0-262-14073-X.
  2. Brodmann, 1909
  3. Walker, 1940
  4. Hoshi, E. (2001). "Functional specialization within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: a review of anatomical and physiological studies of non-human primates.". Neuroscience Research. 54 (2): 73–84. doi:10.1016/j.neures.2005.10.013. PMID 16310877.
  5. Mylius, V. (2013). "Definition of DLPFC and M1 according to anatomical landmarks for navigated brain stimulation: inter-rater reliability, accuracy, and influence of gender and age.". NeuroImage. 78: 224–32. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.061. PMID 23567888.
  6. Cieslik, E. (2013). "Is There "One" DLPFC in Cognitive Action Control? Evidence for Heterogeneity From Co-Activation-Based Parcellation". Cerebral Cortex. 23 (11): 2677–2689. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhs256.
  7. 1 2 3 4 Moss, Simmon. "Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex". Psychlopedia. Retrieved 11 November 2013.
  8. Lee, T. G.; Blumenfeld, R. S.; d'Esposito, M. (2013). "Disruption of Dorsolateral but Not Ventrolateral Prefrontal Cortex Improves Unconscious Perceptual Memories". Journal of Neuroscience. 33 (32): 13233–7. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5652-12.2013. PMC 3735892Freely accessible. PMID 23926275.
  9. 1 2 3 4 Editors: Bruce L. Miller & Jeffrey L. Cummings (2007). The Human Frontal Lobes: Functions and Disorders. The Guilford Press. p. 355. ISBN 1-59385-329-7.
  10. 1 2 James B. Hale; Catherine A. Fiorello (2004). School neuropsychology: A Practitioner's Handbook. Guilford Press. pp. 64–65. ISBN 1593850115.
  11. Goldman-Rakic, Patricia S. (1995). "Architecture of the Prefrontal Cortex and the Central Executive". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 769: 71–83. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1995.tb38132.x. PMID 8595045.
  12. Elliott R (2003). Executive functions and their disorders. British Medical Bulletin. (65); 49–59
  13. Monsell S (2003). "Task switching". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 7 (3): 134–140. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00028-7. PMID 12639695.
  14. Chan, R. C. K., Shum, D., Toulopoulou, T. & Chen, E. Y. H., R; Shum, D; Toulopoulou, T; Chen, E (2008). "Assessment of executive functions: Review of instruments and identification of critical issues". Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2. 23 (2): 201–216. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2007.08.010. PMID 18096360.
  15. 1 2 Goldman-Rakic, edited by Geraldine Dawson, Kurt W. Fischer ; foreword by Patricia S. (1994). Human behavior and the developing brain. New York: Guilford Press. ISBN 0898620929.
  16. 1 2 3 4 Carter, Rita (1999). Mapping the mind. Berkeley: University of California Press. ISBN 978-0520224612.
  17. 1 2 3 4 Miller, Bruce L. (1999). The Human Frontal Lobes. New York, New York: The Guilford Press.
  18. Greene, J. D.; Sommerville, RB; Nystrom, LE; Darley, JM; Cohen, JD (2001). "An fMRI Investigation of Emotional Engagement in Moral Judgment". Science. 293 (5537): 2105–8. doi:10.1126/science.1062872. PMID 11557895.
  19. Duncan, John; Owen, Adrian M (2000). "Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands". Trends in Neurosciences. 23 (10): 475–83. doi:10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01633-7. PMID 11006464.
  20. Knoch, D.; Fehr, E. (2007). "Resisting the Power of Temptations: The Right Prefrontal Cortex and Self-Control". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1104 (1): 123–34. doi:10.1196/annals.1390.004. PMID 17344543.
  21. 1 2 Barbey AK, Koenigs M, Grafman J (May 2013). "Dorsolateral prefrontal contributions to human working memory" (PDF). Cortex. 49 (5): 1195–1205. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2012.05.022. PMID 22789779.
  22. 1 2 3 Hemby, edited by S.E.; Bahn, S. (2006). Functional genomics and proteomics in the clinical neurosciences (1. ed.). Amsterdam: Academic Press. ISBN 0444518533.
  23. Knight, ed. by Donald T. Stuss ; Robert T. (2002). Principles of frontal lobe function ([Online-Ausg.]. ed.). Oxford [u.a.]: Oxford Univ. Press. ISBN 0195134974.
  24. Smith, E. E.; Jonides, J.; Koeppe, R. A. (1996). "Dissociating Verbal and Spatial Working Memory Using PET". Cerebral Cortex. 6 (1): 11–20. doi:10.1093/cercor/6.1.11.
  25. Murphy, D. G. M., Daly, E. M., Van Amelsvoort, T., Robertson, D., Simmons, A., & Critchley, H. D. (1998). Functional neuroanatomical dissociation of verbal, visual and spatial working memory. Schizophrenia Research. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0920-9964(97)88566-0
  26. Reuter-Lorenz; Jonides, J.; Smith, E. E.; Hartley, A.; Miller, A.; Marshuetz, C.; Koeppe (2000). "Age differences in the frontal lateralization of verbal and spatial working memory revealed by PET". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 12 (1): 174–187. doi:10.1162/089892900561814.
  27. Ito, Ayahito; Abe, Nobuhito; Fujii, Toshikatsu; Hayashi, Akiko; Ueno, Aya; Mugikura, Shunji; Takahashi, Shoki; Mori, Etsuro (2012). "The contribution of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex to the preparation for deception and truth-telling". Brain Research. 1464: 43–52. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2012.05.004. PMID 22580084.
  28. Karton, Inga; Bachmann, Talis (2011). "Effect of prefrontal transcranial magnetic stimulation on spontaneous truth-telling". Behavioural Brain Research. 225 (1): 209–14. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2011.07.028. PMID 21807030. Lay summary New Scientist (September 6, 2011).
  29. Mansouri, F. A.; Buckley, M. J.; Tanaka, K. (2007). "Mnemonic Function of the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex in Conflict-Induced Behavioral Adjustment". Science. 318 (5852): 987–90. doi:10.1126/science.1146384. PMID 17962523.
  30. 1 2 3 Mansouri, Farshad A.; Tanaka, Keiji; Buckley, Mark J. (February 2009). "Conflict-induced behavioural adjustment: a clue to the executive functions of the prefrontal cortex". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 10 (2): 141–152. doi:10.1038/nrn2538.
  31. 1 2 Kane, Michael J.; Engle, Randall W. (2002). "The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual-differences perspective". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 9 (4): 637–71. doi:10.3758/BF03196323. PMID 12613671.
  32. 1 2 3 4 5 Shackman, Alexander; Brenton W. Mcmenamin; Jeffrey S. Maxwell; Lawrence L. Greischar; Richard J. Davidson (April 2009). "Right Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortical Activity and Behavioral Inhibition". Psychological Science. 20: 1500–1506. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02476.x.
  33. Van Den Bos, W.; Van Dijk, E.; Westenberg, M.; Rombouts, S. A. R. B.; Crone, E. A. (2010). "Changing Brains, Changing Perspectives: The Neurocognitive Development of Reciprocity". Psychological Science. 22 (1): 60–70. doi:10.1177/0956797610391102. PMID 21164174.
  34. Petrican, Raluca; Schimmack, Ulrich (2008). "The role of dorsolateral prefrontal function in relationship commitment". Journal of Research in Personality. 42 (4): 1130–5. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2008.03.001.
  35. Yang, Yang; Paspalas, Constantinos D.; Jin, Lu E.; Picciotto, Marina R.; Arnsten, Amy F. T.; Wang, Min (2013). "Nicotinic α7 receptors enhance NMDA cognitive circuits in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 110 (29): 12078–83. doi:10.1073/pnas.1307849110. PMC 3718126Freely accessible. PMID 23818597.
  36. Callicott, Joseph H. "Physiological Dysfunction of the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex in Schizophrenia Revisited". Retrieved 15 November 2013.
  37. 1 2 Koenigs, Michael, Grafmanb, Jordan. "The functional neuroanatomy of depression: Distinct roles for ventromedial and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex". Behavioural Brain Research. 201: 239–243. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2009.03.004.
  38. Cite error: The named reference Functional_Genomics was invoked but never defined
  39. 1 2 3 4 Qin, Shaozheng; Hermans, Erno J.; Van Marle, Hein J.F.; Luo, Jing; Fernández, Guillén (2009). "Acute Psychological Stress Reduces Working Memory-Related Activity in the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex". Biological Psychiatry. 66 (1): 25–32. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.03.006. PMID 19403118.
  40. Cohen, H.; Kaplan, Z; Kotler, M; Kouperman, I; Moisa, R; Grisaru, N (2004). "Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation of the Right Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: A Double-Blind, Placebo-Controlled Study". American Journal of Psychiatry. 161 (3): 515–24. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.161.3.515. PMID 14992978.
  41. 1 2 3 Gowin, Joshua L., Mackey, Scott, Paulus, Martin P. "Altered risk-related processing in substance users: Imbalance of pain and gain". Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 132: 13–21. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.03.019.
  42. Luria, ed. by K.H. Pribram, A.R. (1973). Psychophysiology of the frontal lobes. New York: Academic Press. ISBN 0125643403.
  43. 1 2 Abernathy, Kenneth, Chandler, L. Judson, Wooward, John J. (2010). "ALCOHOL AND THE PREFRONTAL CORTEX". Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 91: 289–320. doi:10.1016/S0074-7742(10)91009-X. PMC 3593065Freely accessible. PMID 20813246.
Wikimedia Commons has media related to Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.
This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the 11/8/2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.